Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2xdlg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-23T11:25:16.918Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Testing the Identification/Production Hypothesis of Implicit Memory in Schizophrenia: The Role of Response Competition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 December 2015

Valéria R.S. Marques
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy CAPES Foundation, Ministry of Education of Brazil, SetorBancárioNorte, Brasília, DF – Brazil
Pietro Spataro
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
Vincenzo Cestari
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy “Daniel Bovet” Center, Sapienza University Rome, Italy Cell Biology and NeurobiologyInstitute, CNR, Rome, Italy
Antonio Sciarretta
Affiliation:
Acute Psychiatric Care Unit, Department of Mental Health RM-G, San Giovanni Evangelista Hospital, Tivoli, Italy
Clelia Rossi-Arnaud*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Clelia Rossi-Arnaud, Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Via dei Marsi 78, 00185. E-mail: clelia.rossi-arnaud@uniroma1.it

Abstract

Objectives: Previous evidence indicates that patients with schizophrenia exhibit reduced repetition priming in production tasks (in which each response cue engenders a competition between alternative responses), but not in identification tasks (in which each response cue allows a unique response). However, cross-task comparisons may lead to inappropriate conclusions, because implicit tests vary on several dimensions in addition to the critical dimension of response competition. The present study sought to isolate the role of response competition, by varying the number of solutions in the context of the same implicit tasks. Methods: Two experiments investigated the performance of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls in the high-competition and low-competition versions of word-stem completion (Exp.1) and verb generation (Exp.2). Results: Response competition affected both the proportions of stems completed (higher to few-solution than to many-solution stems) and the reaction times of verb generation (slower to nouns having no dominant verb associates than to nouns having one dominant verb associate). Patients with schizophrenia showed significant (non-zero) priming in both experiments: crucially, the magnitude of this facilitation was equivalent to that observed in healthy controls and was not reduced in the high-competition versions of the two tasks. Conclusions: These findings suggest that implicit memory is spared in schizophrenia, irrespective of the degree of response competition during the retrieval phase; in addition, they add to the ongoing debate regarding the validity of the identification/production hypothesis of repetition priming. (JINS, 2015, 21, 314–321)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barch, D.M., & Ceaser, A. (2012). Cognition in schizophrenia: Core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 2734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertinetto, P.M., Burani, C., Laudanna, A., Marconi, L., Ratti, D., Rolando, C., &Thornton, A.M. (2005). Corpus e Lessico di Frequenza dell’Italiano Scritto (CoLFIS). Retrieved from: http://linguistica.sns.it/CoLFIS/Home.htm.Google Scholar
Clare, L., McKenna, P.J., Mortimer, A.M., & Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Memory in schizophrenia: What is impaired and what is preserved? Neuropsychologia, 31, 12251241. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(93)90070-G Google Scholar
Desmond, J.E., Gabrieli, J.D., & Glover, G.H. (1998). Dissociation of frontal and cerebellar activity in a cognitive task: Evidence for a distinction between selection and search. Neuroimage, 7(4), 368376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franks, J.J., Bilbrey, C.W., Lien, K.G., & McNamara, T.P. (2000). Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) and repetition priming. Memory & Cognition, 28(7), 11401151. doi:10.3758/BF03211815 Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J.E., Vaidya, C.J., Stone, M., Francis, W.S., Thompson-Schill, S.L., Fleischman, D.A., & Wilson, R.S. (1999). Convergent behavioral and neuropsychological evidence for a distinction between identification and production forms of repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(4), 479498. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.4.479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geraci, L. (2006). A test of the frontal lobe functioning hypothesis of age deficits in production priming. Neuropsychology, 20(5), 539.Google Scholar
Geraci, L., & Hamilton, M. (2009). Examining the response competition hypothesis of age effects in implicit memory. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 16(6), 683707. doi:10.1080/13825580902912713 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazes, M., Berthet, L., Danion, J.M., Amado, I., Willard, D., Robert, P., && Poirier, M.F. (1999). Impairment of consciously controlled use of memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 13, 5461. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.1.54 Google Scholar
Kay, S.R., Opler, L.A., & Lindenmayer, J. (1988). Reliability and validity of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale for schizophrenics. Psychiatry Research, 23(1), 99110. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(88)90038-8 Google Scholar
Kern, R., Hartzell, A., Izaguirre, B., & Hamilton, A. (2010). Declarative and nondeclarative memory in schizophrenia: What is impaired? What is spared? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 10171027. doi:10.1080/13803391003671166 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Light, L.L., Prull, M.W., La Voie, D.J., & Healy, M.R. (2000). Dual-process theories of memory in old age. In T.J. Perfect & E.A. Maylor (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 238–300). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marques, V.S., Spataro, P., Cestari, V., Sciarretta, A., Iannarelli, F., & Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2015). Is conceptual implicit memory impaired in schizophrenia? Evidence from lexical decision and category verification. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 20(1), 4152. doi:10.1080/13546805.2014.957380 Google Scholar
Marvel, C.L., Schwartz, B.L., & Isaacs, K.L. (2004). Word production deficits in schizophrenia. Brain and Language, 89(1), 182191. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00366-3 Google Scholar
Mulligan, N.W. (2008). Implicit memory. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Mulligan, N.W., & Hartman, M. (1996). Divided attention and indirect memory tests. Memory & Cognition, 24(4), 453465.Google Scholar
Mulligan, N.W., & Peterson, D. (2008). Attention and implicit memory in the category-verification and lexical decision tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, And Cognition, 34(3), 662679. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.662 Google Scholar
Perry, W., Light, G.A., Davis, H., & Braff, D.L. (2000). Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a dissociation on declarative and non-declarative memory tests. Schizophrenia Research, 46(2-3), 167174. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00229-7 Google Scholar
Prull, M.W. (2004). Exploring the identification-production hypothesis of repetition priming in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 108.Google Scholar
Prull, M.W. (2010). Age-related influences on repetition priming in the verb generation task: Examining the role of response competition. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 17(4), 439461. doi:10.1080/13825580903469846 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prull, M.W. (2013). Attention and repetition priming in the verb generation task. Acta Psychologica, 143(2), 218226. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.03.010 Google Scholar
Randolph, C., Gold, J.M., Carpenter, C.J., Goldberg, T.E., & Weinberger, D.R. (1993). Implicit memory in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls: Effects of task demands on susceptibility to priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 853866. doi:10.1080/01688639308402603 Google Scholar
Ruiz, J., Soler, M., Fuentes, I., & Tomás, P. (2007). Intellectual functioning and memory deficits in schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 48, 276282. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.11.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez, P., Ojeda, N., Pena, J., Elizagarate, E., Yoller, A.B., Gutierrez, M., && Ezcurra, J. (2009). Predictors of longitudinal changes in schizophrenia: The role of processing speed. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70, 888896. doi:10.4088/JCP.08m0429419422757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, B., Rosse, R., & Deutsch, S. (1993). Limits of the processing view in accounting for dissociations among memory measures in a clinical population. Memory and Cognition, 21, 6372. doi:10.3758/BF03211165 Google Scholar
Seger, C.A., Rabin, L.A., Desmond, J.E., & Gabrieli, J.D. (1999). Verb generation priming involves conceptual implicit memory. Brain and Cognition, 41(2), 150177.Google Scholar
Silverstein, S.M., Bakshi, S., Nuernberger, S., Carpinello, K., & Wilkniss, S. (2005). Effects of stimulus structure and target-distracter similarity on the development of visual memory representations in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(3), 215229. doi:10.1080/13546800444000029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soler, M., Ruiz, J., Dasi, C., & Fuentes-Durá, I. (2015). Implicit memory functioning in schizophrenia: Explaining inconsistent findings of word stem completion tasks. Psychiatry Research, 226, 347351. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.01.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soler, M., Ruiz, J., Vargas, M., Dasí, C., & Fuentes, I. (2011). Perceptual priming in schizophrenia evaluated by word fragment and word stem completion. Psychiatry Research, 190, 167171. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.08.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spataro, P., Cestari, V., & Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2011). The relationship between divided attention and implicit memory: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 329339. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spataro, P., Mulligan, N.W., & Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2010). Effects of divided attention in the word-fragment completion task with unique and multiple solutions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(1), 1845. doi:10.1080/09541440802685979 Google Scholar
Sponheim, S., Steele, V., & McGuire, K. (2004). Verbal memory processes in schizophrenia patients and biological relatives of schizophrenia patients: Intact implicit memory impaired explicit recollection. Schizophrenia Research, 71, 339348. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.04.008 Google Scholar
Stone, M., Ladd, S.L., Vaidya, C.J., & Gabrieli, J.E. (1998). Word-identification priming for ignored and attended words. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 7(2), 238258. doi:10.1006/ccog.1998.0326.Google Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S.L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G.K., & Farah, M.J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: A reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, 1479214797. doi:940569210.1073/pnas.94.26.14792 Google Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S.L., & Kan, I.P. (2001). Perceptual and conceptual sources of priming on a word generation task. Memory & Cognition, 29(5), 698706.Google Scholar
Vaidya, C., Gabrieli, J.E., Keane, M.M., Monti, L.A., Gutiérrez-Rivas, H., & Zarella, M.M. (1997). Evidence for multiple mechanisms of conceptual priming on implicit memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 13241343. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.6.1324 Google Scholar
Woodward, N.D., Duffy, B., & Karbasforoushan, H. (2013). Prefrontal cortex activity during response predicts processing speed impairment in schizophrenia. Journal of The International Neuropsychological Society, 19(7), 782791. doi:10.1017/S1355617713000532 Google Scholar