Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-9q27g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-18T01:05:02.588Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rey Complex Figure: Figural and spatial memory before and after temporal lobectomy for intractable epilepsy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 May 2007

ANTHONY C. KNEEBONE
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and School of Medicine, Flinders Medical Centre, Adelaide, Australia. Department of Neurology, Queen Elizabeth Hospital, Adelaide, Australia.
GREGORY P. LEE
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Medical College of Georgia, Augusta, Georgia.
LEE T. WADE
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Medical College of Georgia, Augusta, Georgia.
DAVID W. LORING
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida. Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida.

Abstract

Reliable neuropsychological markers of right temporal integrity have proven elusive. Specifically it is unclear whether figural and spatial aspects of visual memory are differentially affected by right temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) and subsequent resection. To investigate this we used the modified Rey Complex Figure (RCF) scoring system devised by Brier et al. (1996) to obtain separate indices of figural and spatial memory in TLE surgery candidates. We extended on their study by examining presurgical performance and change following right and left temporal lobectomy (RATL, n = 38, LATL, n = 42) in individuals from a cross-institutional sample with and without hippocampal sclerosis (HS+/HS−). Contrary to expectation neither figural nor spatial RCF recall were differentially sensitive to RTLE, right HS, or subsequent resection. Presurgically, laterality effects on both figural and spatial memory indices were not found although HS− individuals significantly outperformed HS+ individuals on both measures. Following surgery the largest decrements in both figural and spatial recall were observed among LATL HS− participants. We concluded that RCF recall is a poor marker of right temporal lobe function and suggest it may be a “surrogate” measure of left temporal lobe function possibly due to the verbalizability of many of its components. (JINS, 2007, 13, 664–671.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abrahams, S., Pickering, A., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1997). Spatial memory deficits in patients with unilateral damage to the right hippocampal formation. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1124.Google Scholar
Barr, W.B. (1997). Examining the right temporal lobe's role in nonverbal memory. Brain and Cognition, 35, 2641.Google Scholar
Barr, W.B., Chelune, G.J., Hermann, B.P., Loring, D.W., Perrine, K., Strauss, E., Trenerry, M.R., & Westerveld, M. (1997). The use of figural reproduction tests as measures of nonverbal memory in epilepsy surgery candidates. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 435443.Google Scholar
Baxendale, S.A., Thompson, P.J., & van Paesschen, W. (1998). A test of spatial memory and its clinical utility in the pre-surgical investigation of temporal lobe epilepsy patients. Neuropsychologia, 36, 591602.Google Scholar
Brier, J.I., Plenger, P.M., Castillo, R., Fuchs, K., Wheless, J.W., Thomas, A.B., Brookshire, B.L., Willmore, L.J., & Papanicolaou, A. (1996). Effects of temporal lobe epilepsy on spatial and figural aspects of memory for a complex geometric figure. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 535540.Google Scholar
Chelune, G.J. & Najm, I.M. (2001). Risk factors associated with postsurgical decrements in memory. In H.O. Lüders & Y.G. Comair (Eds.), Epilepsy surgery (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Dobbins, I.G., Kroll, N.E.A., Tulving, E., Knight, R.T., & Gazzaniga, M. (1998). Unilateral medial temporal lobe memory impairment: Type deficit, function deficit, or both? Neuropsychologia, 36, 115127.Google Scholar
Donofrio, N., Glosser, G., & Saykin, A. (1999, February). Memory for faces dissociates from memory for location following right anterior temporal lobectomy. Paper presented at the 27th Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Boston, Massachusetts.
Griffith, H.R., Pyzalski, R.W., O'Leary, D.O., Magnotta, V., Bell, B., Dow, C., Hermann, B., & Seidenberg, M. (2003). A controlled quantitative MRI volumetric investigation of hippocampal contributions to immediate and delayed memory performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 11171127.Google Scholar
Haxby, J.V., Grady, C.L., Horwitz, B., Ungerlieder, L.G., Mishkin, M., Carson, R.E., Herscovitch, P., Shapiro, M.B., & Rapoport, S.I. (1991). Dissociation of spatial and object visual processing in human extrastriate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 88, 16211625.Google Scholar
Haxby, J.V., Grady, C.L., Horwitz, B., Salerno, J., Ungerlieder, L.G., Mishkin, M., & Schapiro, M.B. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. In B. Gulya, B.D. Ottoson, & P.E. Roland (Eds.), Functional organization of human visual cortex. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.
Helmstaedter, C., Pohl, C., & Elger, C.E. (1995). Relations between verbal and nonverbal memory performance: Evidence of confounding effects particularly in patients with right temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex, 31, 345355.Google Scholar
Hermann, B.P., Seidenberg, M., Wyler, A., & Haltiner, A. (1993). Dissociation of object recognition and spatial localization abilities following temporal lobe lesions in humans. Neuropsychology, 7, 343350.Google Scholar
Hermann, B.P., Wyler, A.R., Somes, G., Berry, A.D., & Dohan, F.C. (1992). Pathological status of the mesial temporal lobe predicts memory outcome after anterior temporal lobectomy. Neurosurgery, 31, 652657.Google Scholar
Kilpatrick, C., Murrie, V., Cook, M., Andrewes, D., Desmond, P., & Hopper, J. (1997). Degree of left hippocampal atrophy correlates with severity of neuropsychological deficits. Seizure, 6, 213218.Google Scholar
Kneebone, A.C. (2001). Presurgical neuropsychological evaluation for localization and lateralization of the epileptogenic zone. In H.O. Lüders & Y.G. Comair (Eds.), Epilepsy surgery (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Kneebone, A.C., Chelune, G.J., Dinner, D.S., Naugle, R.I., & Awad, I.A. (1995). Intracarotid amobarbital procedure as a predictor of material-specific memory change after anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia, 36, 857865.Google Scholar
Köhler, S., Moscovitch, M., & Melo, B. (2001). Episodic memory for object location versus episodic memory for object identity: Do they rely on distinct encoding processes? Memory and Cognition, 29, 948959.Google Scholar
Köhler, S., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Houle, S., & McIntosh, A.R. (1998). Networks of domain-specific and general regions involved in episodic memory for spatial location and object identity. Neuropsychologia, 36, 129142.Google Scholar
Lacritz, L.H., Barnard, H.D., Van Ness, P., Agostini, M., Diaz-Arrastia, R., & Cullum, C.M. (2004). Qualitative analysis of WMS-III Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction in temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 521530.Google Scholar
Lee, G.P., Loring, D.W., & Thompson, J.L. (1989). Construct validity of material-specific memory measures following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Psychological Assessment, 1, 192197.Google Scholar
Lee, T.M.C., Yip, J.T.H., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2002). Memory deficits after resection from left or right anterior temporal lobe in humans: A meta-analytic review. Epilepsia, 43, 283291.Google Scholar
Lencz, T., McCarthy, G., Bronen, R.A., Scott, T.M., Inserni, J.A., Sass, K.J., Novelly, R.A., Jung, H.K., & Spencer, D.D. (1992). Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging in temporal lobe epilepsy: Relationship to neuropathology and neuropsychological function. Annals of Neurology, 31, 629637.Google Scholar
Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., & Loring, D.W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Loring, D.W., Meador, K.J., Lee, G.P., King, D.W., Nichols, M.E., Park, Y.D., Murro, A.M., Gallagher, B.B., & Smith, J.R. (1995). Wada memory asymmetries predict verbal memory decline after anterior temporal lobectomy. Neurology, 45, 13291333.Google Scholar
McConley, R., Martin, R., Baños, J., Blanton, P., & Faught, E. (2006). Global/local scoring modifications for the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: Relation to unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy patients. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 383390.Google Scholar
McDade, L.A. & Jones-Gotman, M. (2001). Face learning and memory: The twins test. Neuropsychology, 15, 525534.Google Scholar
McDermid Vaz, S.A. (2004). Nonverbal memory functioning following right anterior temporal lobectomy: A meta-analytic review. Seizure, 7, 446452.Google Scholar
Moscovitch, M., Kapur, S., Köhler, S., & Houle, S. (1995). Distinct neural correlates of visual long-term memory for spatial location and object identity: A positron emission tomography study in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 337213725.Google Scholar
Newman, P.D. & Krikorian, R. (2001). Encoding and complex figure recall. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 728733.Google Scholar
Nunn, J.A., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1998). Selective spatial memory impairment after right unilateral temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia, 36, 837848.Google Scholar
Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A.R., Cabeza, R., Habib, R., Houle, S., & Tulving, E. (1996). General and specific brain regions involved in encoding and retrieval of events: What, where, and when. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 1128011285.Google Scholar
O'Craven, K.M., Downing, P.E., & Kanwisher, N. (1999). fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature, 401, 584587.Google Scholar
Owen, A., Milner, B., Petrides, M., & Evans, A.C. (1996). Memory for object features versus memory for object location: A positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 92129217.Google Scholar
Piguet, O., Saling, M.M., O'Shea, M.F., Berkovic, S.F., & Bladin, P.F. (1994). Rey figure distortions reflect nonverbal differences between right and left foci in unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 451460.Google Scholar
Rausch, R. (1991). Effects of temporal lobe surgery on behavior. Advances in Neurology, 55, 279292.Google Scholar
Rausch, R. & Babb, T.L. (1993). Hippocampal neuron loss and memory scores before and after temporal lobe surgery for epilepsy. Archives of Neurology, 5, 812817.Google Scholar
Redoblado, M.A., Grayson, S.J., & Miller, L.A. (2003). Lateralized temporal-lobe-lesion effects on learning and memory: Examining the contributions of stimulus novelty and presentation mode. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 3648.Google Scholar
Sass, K.J., Sass, A., Westerveld, M., Lencz, T., Novelly, R.A., Kim, J.H., & Spencer, D.D. (1992). Specificity in the correlation of verbal memory and hippocampal neuron loss: Dissociation of memory, language, and verbal intellectual ability. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 662672.Google Scholar
Sass, K.J., Westerveld, M., Buchanan, C.P., Spencer, S.S., Kim, J.H., & Spencer, D.D. (1994). Degree of hippocampal neuron loss determines severity of verbal memory decrease after left anteromesiotemporal lobectomy. Epilepsia, 35, 11791186.Google Scholar
Sawrie, S.M., Chelune, G.J., Naugle, R.I., & Lüders, H.O. (1996). Empirical methods for assessing meaningful change following epilepsy surgery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 556564.Google Scholar
Saykin, A.J., Robinson, L.J., Stafiniak, P., Kester, D.B., Gur, R.C., O'Connor, M.J., & Sperling, M.R. (1992). Neuropsychological changes after anterior temporal lobectomy: Acute effects on memory, language and music. In T.L. Bennett (Ed.), The neuropsychology of epilepsy. New York: Plenum.
Sutherland, R.J. & Rudy, J.W. (1989). Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia. Psychobiology, 17, 129144.Google Scholar
Trenerry, M.R., Jack, C.R., Jr., Ivnik, R.J., Sharbrough, F.W., Cascino, G.D., Hirschorn, K.A., Marsh, W.R., Kelly, P.J., & Meyer, F.B. (1993). MRI hippocampal volumes and memory function before and after temporal lobectomy. Neurology, 43, 18001805.Google Scholar
Zakzanis, K.K. (2001). Statistics to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth: Formulae, illustrative numerical examples, and heuristic interpretation of effects size analyses for neuropsychological researchers. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 653667.Google Scholar