Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T18:10:47.384Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parkinson’s disease disrupts the ability to initiate and apply episodic foresight

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2022

Sarah P. Coundouris*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
Julie D. Henry
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
Peter G. Rendell
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia School of Behavioural and Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia
Alexander C. Lehn
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Princess Alexandra Hospital, Woolloongabba, Queensland, Australia The University of Queensland, Princess Alexandra Hospital, Clinical School, Woolloongabba, Queensland, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Sarah P. Coundouris, email: s.coundouris@uq.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

While Parkinson’s disease is associated with impairments in many aspects of prospective cognition, no study to date has tested whether these difficulties extend to problems using episodic foresight to guide future-directed behavior. To provide the first examination of whether people with Parkinson’s disease are impaired in their capacity to initiate and apply episodic foresight.

Method:

People with Parkinson’s disease (n = 42), and a demographically matched neurotypical comparison group (n = 42) completed a validated behavioral assessment that met strict criteria for assessing episodic foresight (Virtual Week-Foresight), as well as a broader neurocognitive and clinical test battery.

Results:

People with Parkinson’s disease were significantly less likely than the comparison group to acquire items that would later allow a problem to be solved and were also less likely to subsequently use these items for problem resolution. These deficits were largely unrelated to performance on other cognitive measures or clinical characteristics of the disorder.

Conclusions:

The ability to engage in episodic foresight in an adaptive way is compromised in Parkinson’s disease. This appears to be a stable feature of the disorder, and one that is distinct from other clinical symptoms and neurocognitive deficits. It is now critical to establish exactly why these difficulties exist and how they impact on real-life functional capacity.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2022

In 2020, the worldwide prevalence of Parkinson’s disease (PD) was estimated to be 9.4 million, a rise of 3.4 million from 2016 (Maserejian et al., Reference Maserejian, Vinikoor-Imler and Dilley2020). At the same time, increasing life expectancy has contributed to greater disease duration in more recently diagnosed cohorts (Macleod et al., Reference Macleod, Taylor and Counsell2014). This now makes PD one of the leading causes of disability globally (Dorsey et al., Reference Dorsey, Elbaz, Nichols, Abbasi, Abd-Allah, Adsuar, Abdelalim, Ansha, Brayne, Choi, Collado-Mateo, Dahodwala, Do, Edessa, Endres, Fereshtehnejad, Foreman, Gankpe, Gupta and Murray2018), meaning that it is of unprecedented importance to understand the symptoms particularly contributing to disease burden in this cohort. However, while the impact of motor symptoms on functional capacity has been the focus of considerable research (Chapuis et al., Reference Chapuis, Ouchchane, Metz, Gerbaud and Durif2005; Hariz & Forsgren, Reference Hariz and Forsgren2011; Wressle et al., Reference Wressle, Engstrand and Granérus2007), the role of neurocognitive impairment has received comparatively less attention.

In broader clinical literature, failures of prospective cognition are widely acknowledged to be amongst the most debilitating neurocognitive symptoms because they fundamentally disrupt the ability to anticipate, plan and/or act with the future in mind (Henry, Reference Henry2021). A recent meta-analytic review revealed that PD is associated with moderate to large sized deficits in two key aspects of prospective cognition, planning (forward thinking and the subsequent execution of a sequence of actions) and prospective memory (PM; memory for future intentions, such as remembering to take medication; Coundouris et al., Reference Coundouris, Terrett, Laakso, Schweitzer, Kneebone, Rendell and Henry2020), with such deficits linked to atrophy and dopaminergic depletion of neural structures including prefrontal regions (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Peppe, Caltagirone and Carlesimo2008; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Souchay and Moulin2011). Moreover, this meta-analysis highlighted a surprising omission in this literature, as it found that no study to date had examined whether PD disrupts another important aspect of prospective cognition: the ability to use episodic foresight to guide future-directed behaviors. From a neurological perspective, PD-related impairment in foresight can be anticipated, as there is considerable overlap in the brain regions implicated in both episodic foresight and the neuropathology of PD, including structures in the prefrontal cortex, the medial temporal lobe, and the fronto-parietal control network, (Benoit & Schacter, Reference Benoit and Schacter2015; Bertossi et al., Reference Bertossi, Tesini, Cappelli and Ciaramelli2016; Camicioli et al., Reference Camicioli, Korzan, Foster, Emery, Bastos and Hanstock2004; Klobušiaková et al., Reference Klobušiaková, Mareček, Fousek, Výtvarová and Rektorová2019; Niethammer et al., Reference Niethammer, Feigin and Eidelberg2012; Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Benoit and Szpunar2017; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Chen, Zhang, Xu and Feng2020)

Of the various forms of prospection, episodic foresight has been proposed to be the most flexible and functionally powerful, requiring not only the ability to imagine future scenarios but to also use these imaginings to guide future-directed behavior (Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2008; Suddendorf & Corballis, Reference Suddendorf and Corballis1997). Indeed, this ability to foresee the future has been proposed in the literature as an important determinant for other prospective abilities (Atance & O’Neill, Reference Atance and O’Neill2001; Schacter & Addis, Reference Schacter and Addis2007; Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2008; Suddendorf & Moore, Reference Suddendorf and Moore2011; Terrett et al., Reference Terrett, Rose, Henry, Bailey, Altgassen, Phillips, Kliegel and Rendell2016). Specifically, episodic foresight has been conceptualized as a means of optimizing planning efficiency during the initial selection and development of plans, and has also been regarded as an effective strategy for PM, as simulating a future intended action strengthens the encoding of this behavior during the intention formation stage. Episodic foresight is thus a practical, anticipatory skill, which enhances wellbeing, directs behavior and emotions, and allows for educated, future-directed appraisals (Atance & O’Neill, Reference Atance and O’Neill2001; Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2008; Spreng & Levine, Reference Spreng and Levine2006). Managing finances, going shopping, and food preparation, are some of the many daily activities considered critical for independent living that are reliant on this ability (Suddendorf & Henry, Reference Suddendorf and Henry2013).

Given its complexity, many researchers argue that this skill draws on several distinct and interrelated abilities including episodic memory and executive control. According to the constructive simulation hypothesis, episodic memories provide the foundation to construct future hypothetical events (Addis & Schacter, Reference Addis and Schacter2008), but executive functions have also been implicated as critical to inhibit simply recasting past memories (Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2007; Suddendorf & Corballis, Reference Suddendorf and Corballis2007; Suddendorf & Henry, Reference Suddendorf and Henry2013). That is, episodic foresight also depends on one’s ability to hold and manipulate memories, in order to flexibly recombine past experience.

To date, the broader episodic foresight literature has been dominated by phenomenological paradigms that either rely on the participant rating their experience (such as the vividness of future simulations), or which use more objective measurements to index the quantity of episodic details in future event narratives (see e.g., Gamble et al., Reference Gamble, Moreau, Tippett and Addis2019, Hallford et al., Reference Hallford, Austin, Takano and Raes2018). For the latter, a large amount of episodic detail generated in a future event narrative is presupposed to reflect more detailed pre-experience, and thus a greater capacity for episodic foresight. Indeed, of the one study to assess episodic foresight in people with PD, a purely phenomenological measure was used. Specifically, de Vito et al. (Reference de Vito, Gamboz, Brandimonte, Barone, Amboni and Della Sala2012) prompted participants to mentally pre-experience autobiographical events and showed that the PD group generated significantly fewer episodic details relative to the comparison group, indicative of problems engaging in episodic future thinking. However, while the ability to imagine future scenarios is a fundamental component of episodic foresight, a measure of the ability to use this imagining to appropriately guide future-directed behavior is critical to inform whether problems with episodic foresight actually lead to functional difficulties in everyday life.

The present study therefore aimed to provide the first examination of whether PD-related deficits are evident in the functional application of episodic foresight. In service of this goal, the validated paradigm VW-Foresight was used (Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Corballis and Suddendorf2014), as this is the only behavioral measure available for adults that meets strict criteria for demonstrating episodic foresight (Suddendorf & Corballis, Reference Suddendorf and Corballis2010). The task involves participants’ independently identifying and resolving problems through acquiring and subsequently using items at an appropriate time-point without external, overt cueing (Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Corballis and Suddendorf2014; Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Robinson and Suddendorf2016), therefore making this methodology distinct from the original Virtual Week and other prospection assessments. Specifically, in both the broader planning and PM literatures, the methodology has involved prescribing an action or outcome which is then independently planned or initiated using a rehearsed intention. However, there is no requirement to self-generate an intention as the problem, cue, and intention are explicitly directed by the stimuli or task. For instance, in Kliegel et al. (Reference Kliegel, McDaniel and Einstein2000) study, the primary focus was the extent to which participants developed sophisticated or well-elaborated plans after being provided with a circumscribed, defined set of tasks, and not the capacity to independently identify a problem and self-generate an intention to subsequently solve this. Thus the critical distinction between VW-Foresight and measures of prospective memory and planning is that it does not assess the ability to create or initiate future plans based on provided instructions, but assesses one’s ability to independently identify a future problem and self-generate an intention to resolve this (Suddendorf & Corballis, Reference Suddendorf and Corballis2010).

Given that episodic foresight imposes demands on many of the neural structures and networks known to be affected by PD, and that both phenomenological and functional episodic foresight impairments have been identified in many other clinical groups (e.g., Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Robinson and Suddendorf2016; Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Robinson, Rendell and Suddendorf2019; Mercuri et al., Reference Mercuri, Terrett, Henry, Curran, Elliott and Rendell2018; Terrett et al., Reference Terrett, Lyons, Henry, Ryrie, Suddendorf and Rendell2017), this study was designed to test the preregistered prediction that episodic foresight would be significantly disrupted in people with this disorder. A secondary aim was to establish the cognitive and clinical correlates of any observed episodic foresight difficulties. A broad cognitive battery was used that included a measure of premorbid IQ, as well as measures that have been theorized to either play a role in supporting episodic foresight (i.e., executive function; Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2007; Suddendorf & Corballis, Reference Suddendorf and Corballis2007; Suddendorf & Henry, Reference Suddendorf and Henry2013), or which tap into cognitive constructs that previous literature shows people with PD often experience difficulties with (attention, recognition, and recall; see Kudlicka et al., Reference Kudlicka, Clare and Hindle2011; Watson & Leverenz, Reference Watson and Leverenz2010).

Method

This study formed part of a larger preregistered testing protocol (https://osf.io/rvgh5/?view_only=414e3f3db9d04dfeb19928275ad3650e) that detailed four distinct studies. Only methods relevant to this study are described below.

Participants

The PD group was individuals on stable medication being treated at the Princess Alexandra Hospital. Recruitment for neurotypical (i.e., no neurological disease or neurological development disorder) older adults occurred online (Facebook advert, University of Queensland website), through media outlets (Your Time Magazine, Ageing Mind Initiative Newsletter), and word of mouth. To be eligible participants had to: (1) Be a native English speaker, or have high levels of English proficiency; (2) Have no current or past brain trauma; (3) Have no current/previous diagnosis of a serious psychiatric illness (e.g., bipolar disorder, schizophrenia). In the case of previous anxiety/depression, only those clearly linked to a specific negative life event (e.g., losing a spouse, financial hardship) were considered eligible. For the PD group, if the current anxiety/depression onset occurred after PD-diagnosis these participants were included (although note, those with severe on-going issues involving suicidal ideation, or recent hospitalization were not). For the comparison group, participants could not be receiving treatment for psychiatric illness at the time of testing; (4) Have no current/reoccurring issues with substance abuse including the use of either drugs or alcohol for purposes other than intended or in excessive, uncontrolled amounts; (5) Have no other illness that may impact day-to-day functioning and engagement with activities and; (6) Score 24 or higher on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975).

Fifty PD participants completed the test battery, with seven later excluded due to psychiatric illness (n = 4), brain trauma (n = 1), a combination of brain trauma and psychiatric illness (n = 1), a low score on the MMSE (n = 1), or not understanding the tasks involved (n = 1). The final PD sample consisted of 42 participants (50% Male; age M = 64.81 years, SD = 10.20 years; education M = 13.15 years, SD = 4.16 years; MMSE M = 28.79, SD = 1.22). Table 1 details diagnosis and treatment information for the PD group.

Table 1. Parkinson’s disease related participant information

H&Y = Modified Hoehn and Yahr scale.

a Majority of participants were using multiple treatments.

Disease onset based upon the Parkinson’s Foundation’s classification (see parkinson.org).

Sixty-one neurotypical individuals completed the test battery as a comparison group, with 19 later excluded due to psychiatric illness (n = 1), and to ensure that the two groups were matched demographicallyFootnote 1. The final comparison group comprised 42 participants (50% Male; age M = 65.00 years, SD = 8.92 years; education M = 14.61 years, SD = 3.16 years; MMSE M = 28.79, SD = 1.09; see Supplementary Table 1 for a complete demographic breakdown). Three independent t-tests were completed for age, years of education, and scores on the MMSE and showed that the two groups were demographically matched (ps = .928, .074 and 1.000, respectively).

Materials

VW-foresight

VW-Foresight (Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Corballis and Suddendorf2014) is a computerized game where participants move a token around a board with the role of a die (see Figure 1a). Each circuit constitutes one virtual day, with participants in this study completing one experimenter assisted practice day, and two individually completed test days. There was no time limit on completing the game, with participants taking on average 50 min.

Figure 1. Virtual week-foresight task breakdown.

The task is described in detail in Lyons et al. (Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Corballis and Suddendorf2014). However, in brief, the aim of the game is to: (1) identify problems when they arise; (2) identify and store items that may help to solve these problems; and (3) recognize when the problem context re-arises and use the relevant stored item for problem resolution. For example, each episodic foresight task will begin with a problem embedded in a situation card (Figure 1b). At a later point, the opportunity to acquire an item helpful in solving this problem will occur within a daily activity card (Figure 1c). The chosen item will be stored for later use within “Your Stored Items,” which are accessible both on the board, and within each situation card to allow for use at any time. When participants encounter a situation card in which an initial problem is still apparent (Figure 1d), they may resolve the problem by selecting the item required from “Your Stored Items.”

The key dependent measures are item acquisition and item use. Item acquisition is scored as the number of target items acquired using the daily activities cards. Item use is, however, more complex, and as in previous studies (see Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Corballis and Suddendorf2014), was calculated in two ways. The first is the number of target items correctly used (unconditional item use). Here, an item is scored as correctly used when the item is selected for use while on the target situation card (i.e., the situation card that provides the context for the related problem to be resolved). However, given that the ability to use an item is contingent on first acquiring that item, another measure of item use (conditional item use) was also calculated, in which the number of items correctly used was first conditionalized on initial acquisition (i.e., the number of used items is divided by the number acquired, to produce a proportion of already acquired items).

Background measures

A broader neurocognitive test battery was also administered, which consisted of the Digit Span Test (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008; forward span a measure of attention, and backward span a measure of working memory), phonemic and semantic Verbal Fluency (a measure of executive function; Henry & Crawford, Reference Henry and Crawford2004), the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt & Benedict, Reference Brandt and Benedict2001; a measure of verbal learning and recall; retention and recognition discrimination index scores calculated), and the National Adult Reading Test-Second Edition (NART; Nelson & Willison, Reference Nelson and Willison1991; an indicator of premorbid IQ).

Validated self and informant rated clinical measures were also administered: The Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES; Marin et al., Reference Marin, Biedrzycki and Firinciogullari1991), the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond & Snaith, Reference Zigmond and Snaith1983), the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI-Q; Kaufer et al., Reference Kaufer, Cummings, Ketchel, Smith, MacMillan, Shelley, Lopez and DeKosky2000), the Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire-39 (PDQ-39; Peto et al., Reference Peto, Jenkinson, Fitzpatrick and Greenhall1995), and the Parkinson’s Disease Sleep Scale (PDSS-2; Trenkwalder et al., Reference Trenkwalder, Kohnen, Högl, Metta, Sixel-Döring, Frauscher, Hülsmann, Martinez-Martin and Chaudhuri2011). For each of these measures higher scores are indicative of greater clinical symptoms (apathy, negative affect, psychiatric illness, PD severity, and sleep disturbances, respectively).

Procedure

The procedure complied with the Declaration of Helsinki for medical research involving human subjects and was approved by the Human Research Ethics committee of The University of Queensland (Approval No. 2018001920). The measures described in this paper formed part of a larger protocol completed in person, one-on-one, with no limit on the frequency or length of breaks. Participants had the option to complete testing at the University of Queensland, at Hospital, or at their home. Four PD participants elected to complete this testing over two days. All participants were provided with an information sheet outlining the purpose of the study, and the ethical testing and handling of responses. The information sheet was also discussed verbally with the researcher, before participants provided written consent.

As most neurocognitive and clinical assessments were administered using a laptop, prior to commencing the researcher gauged participants’ comfort in controlling the computer mouse. Based on this appraisal, the computer mouse was partially/completely controlled by the researcher in 24 instances (PD Group = 19, Comparison Group = 5; i.e., due to shaking, bradykinesia, unfamiliarityFootnote 2). In these circumstances, the only difference to testing was that participants verbally, rather than physically, responded to questions/instructed the examiner how to move the mouse.

At the conclusion of testing all participants were reimbursed $80 in Gift Cards for time and travel expenses. This study coincided with the COVID-19 pandemic (commenced January 2020, concluded April 2021), and consequently small changes were made to the testing session to be in line with current restrictions at time. This included a mask being worn by the researcher and/or participant, a second screen was introduced to help maintain a safe distance between the researcher and participant, hand sanitizer was provided, and all material cleaned after each session.

Statistical analyses

The Hmisc (Harrell, Reference Harrell2021), ggpubr (Kassambara, Reference Kassambara2020), rstatix (Kassambara, Reference Kassambara2021), and tidyverse (Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGowan, François, Grolemund, Hayes, Henry, Hester, Kuhn, Pedersen, Miller, Bache, Müller, Ooms, Robinson, Seidel, Spinu and Yutani2019) packages were used within RStudio (version 4.0.0; RStudio Team, 2020) to analyze the data. For VW-Foresight, mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted, with foresight task (percentage of item acquired, percentage of items used) as the within-subjects factor, and group as the between-subjects factor. Pearson’s correlations were calculated to explore cognitive and clinical correlates of VW-Foresight in the PD group. All statistical analyses were two-tailed and ps < .05 considered significant.

Missing data only occurred in the informant rated NPI-Q (severity cases = 5, distress cases = 4; See Supplementary Table 2 for a complete informant breakdown). In these instances, the mean response from that participant group was inputted. In the 27 cases where two responses were provided, the following sequential steps were taken to choose one informant: (1) fewer missing data; (2) partner over other relationships; (3) most regular physical contact; and (4) relationship length.

Results

For VW-Foresight data, the first ANOVA (in which item use was unconditionalized), revealed a main effect of group, F(1, 82) = 17.20, p < .001, η p2 = 0.17, indicating that the PD group were impaired overall, Ms = 50.68% and 69.90%, SDs = 33.81% and 29.34% respectively. A main effect of task was also found, F(1, 82) = 211.97, p < .001, η p2 = 0.72, which reflected overall performance being greater for item acquisition relative to item use, Ms = 80.10% and 40.48%, SDs = 19.21% and 32.05% respectively. However, there was no interaction between the group and task, F(1, 82) = 2.85, p = .095, η p2 = 0.03.

The second ANOVA with conditionalized item use returned an identical pattern of results, with a main effect for group (F(1, 82) = 15.08, p < .001, η p2 = 0.16; Ms = 54.43% and 72.64%, SDs = 34.25% and 28.26%) and task (F(1, 82) = 91.55, p < .001, η p2 = 0.53; Ms = 46.98% and 80.10%, SDs = 34.86% and 19.21%), but no interaction between the two (F(1, 82) = 1.07, p = .303, η p2 = 0.01). These data are displayed visually in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Performance on virtual week-foresight for Parkinson’s disease and comparison group participants. Bars represent one standard error of the mean.

Finally, it can be seen in Table 2 that no clinical measures were correlated with item acquisition or use in either of the two groups (see Supplementary Table 3 for descriptive statistics). The only cognitive correlate to emerge was between item acquisition and verbal fluency, which emerged as a moderate sized association in both groups. The only cognitive correlate to emerge for both groups was between item acquisition and verbal fluency, which emerged as a moderate sized association.

Table 2. Pearson’s bivariate correlations of background cognitive and clinical measures with virtual week-foresight (unconditional item use)

AES = apathy evaluation scale; HADS = the hospital anxiety and depression scale; HVLT-R = Hopkins verbal learning test-revised; NART = The national adult reading test; NPI-Q = the neuropsychiatric inventory informant rated; PD = Parkinson’s disease; PDQ-39 = Parkinson’s disease questionnaire-39; PDSS-2 = Parkinson’s disease sleep scale revised.

* p < .05.

** p < .001.

n = 42 for each group. Except NPI-Q where n = 20 for Parkinson’s disease and n = 29 for comparison group.

Discussion

The present study makes a novel and important contribution to the broader PD literature by showing for the first time that, in line with preregistered hypotheses, people with PD are significantly and substantially impaired in their capacity to use episodic foresight to guide future behavior. Relative to the comparison group, the PD group were less likely to both initially acquire items needed to solve later problems, as well as to later use these items when the situation was appropriate, with each of these deficits large in magnitude. The latter of these effects also emerged regardless of whether item use was treated as independent of initial item acquisition, or first conditionalized upon it. These data therefore suggest that the PD-related difficulties in episodic foresight are not driven by a particular task demand, but instead reflect broader disruptions in the higher-level abilities that allow for problem identification, potential solution forecasting, and solution implementation. The findings also align with other clinical studies that have used VW-Foresight and identified a reduced capacity in both initial item acquisition as well as later problem resolution (Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Robinson and Suddendorf2016, Reference Lyons, Henry, Robinson, Rendell and Suddendorf2019; Terrett et al., Reference Terrett, Lyons, Henry, Ryrie, Suddendorf and Rendell2017), and therefore provides further support for the view that broad-based problems with episodic foresight may be a common feature of clinical illness.

Given that the ability to initiate and engage in episodic foresight is a critical prerequisite for healthy, safe, and autonomous living (Suddendorf & Henry, Reference Suddendorf and Henry2013), these data also help extend our understanding of how cognitive impairment may contribute meaningfully to disease burden in PD specifically. PD-related impairment in other cognitive domains has previously been shown to limit capacity in several important functional domains including management of finance, medication adherence, and practical decision-making (Kudlicka et al., Reference Kudlicka, Hindle, Spencer and Clare2018). Indeed, a review by Koerts et al. (Reference Koerts, König, Tucha and Tucha2016) delineated how nonmotor symptoms contribute meaningfully to the earlier retirement rates associated with PD. Given that the ability to foresee and initiate preparatory action is essential for independent function, research is now needed to directly test how PD-related episodic foresight disruptions impact individuals’ basic and instrumental daily living skills.

While the secondary aim of this study was to explore potential clinical and cognitive correlates of episodic foresight deficits in PD, the results showed that episodic foresight difficulties occurred quite independently of broader clinical symptoms and were only weakly related to broader cognitive function. The only relationship identified for both groups was with one of the executive control measures (verbal fluency), which was positively correlated with item acquisition. As noted earlier, executive control processes have been argued to play a critical role in successfully engaging episodic foresight (Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2007; Suddendorf & Corballis, Reference Suddendorf and Corballis2007; Suddendorf & Henry, Reference Suddendorf and Henry2013). Interestingly, however, a relationship with our second measure of executive function (Digit Span Backwards) only emerged for item use in the healthy comparison group, suggesting that episodic foresight may be more dependent on some executive resources than others. For instance, it may be that episodic foresight relies less on one’s capacity to store and manipulate information (as measured by Digit Span backwards; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008), and more on one’s ability to engage in effortful self-initiation (as is required in the verbal fluency task; Crawford & Henry, Reference Crawford, Henry, Halligan and Wade2005; Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Light, Parker and Levin1997). Further research is now needed to explore this issue. The only other significant association between cognitive function and episodic foresight to emerge was between verbal learning (retention) and item acquisition. However, again this association emerged only in the healthy comparison group. Given that all other studies that have assessed the relationship between verbal learning and episodic foresight in both clinical and nonclinical samples have failed to identify significant associations (e.g., Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Robinson and Suddendorf2016; Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Robinson, Rendell and Suddendorf2019), this does not appear to be a particularly reliable or robust relationship.

While no other associations emerged, an important caveat is that the exclusion criteria placed on psychiatric illness (e.g., no severely depressed PD participants) and cognitive function (≥24 on MMSE) may have restricted the ability to identify significant correlations. Nevertheless, the key point here is that even in a relatively high functioning PD cohort (in terms of broader psychiatric and cognitive status), generalized episodic foresight impairments emerged that were large in magnitude. These data therefore suggest that problems with episodic foresight may be a relatively consistent feature of PD, and not simply secondary to particular clinical or cognitive symptoms of the disorder, although further work is now needed to see whether specific clinical presentations or comorbidities may be associated with even more prominent impairment.

The behavioral data reported in this study aligns with what is currently known about the neurological underpinnings of episodic foresight. Specifically, many of the neural structures and pathways believed to be critical to engage in foresight (Benoit & Schacter, Reference Benoit and Schacter2015; Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2007, Reference Schacter, Benoit and Szpunar2017), are also disrupted in PD (e.g., Brück et al., Reference Brück, Kurki, Kaasinen, Vahlberg and Rinne2004; Camicioli et al., Reference Camicioli, Korzan, Foster, Emery, Bastos and Hanstock2004; Klobušiaková et al., Reference Klobušiaková, Mareček, Fousek, Výtvarová and Rektorová2019; Niethammer et al., Reference Niethammer, Feigin and Eidelberg2012; Pelizzari et al., Reference Pelizzari, Di Tella, Rossetto, Laganà, Bergsland, Piratstru, Meloni, Nemni and Baglio2020). However, to date, the neural underpinnings of episodic foresight have focused only on the phenomenological aspects of foresight (future thinking). While this represents the foundation of episodic foresight, research is now needed to understand the neural regions that are responsible for allowing future-oriented cognitions and behaviors to be put into action (Miloyan et al., Reference Miloyan, McFarlane and Suddendorf2019). Accordingly, studies are needed that examine how the specific neural changes associated with PD map onto one’s capacity to adaptively use these episodic simulations to guide behavior.

Additionally, in light of evidence suggesting that the ability to adaptively engage in episodic foresight is somewhat compromised in people with PD, an important next step is to now better understand the relationship between episodic foresight and other cognitive skills, including other aspects of future-oriented cognition. Episodic foresight has been conceptualized as both a strategy for enhancing PM (by simulating the future) and as a way of optimizing planning efficiency (how we initially choose and develop a plan; Atance & O’Neill, Reference Atance and O’Neill2001; Schacter & Addis, Reference Schacter and Addis2007; Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2008; Suddendorf & Moore, Reference Suddendorf and Moore2011; Terrett et al., Reference Terrett, Rose, Henry, Bailey, Altgassen, Phillips, Kliegel and Rendell2016). PD-related deficits in episodic foresight might therefore contribute to the reduced capacity for PM and planning often seen in this cohort – and this represents an interesting avenue for future research to pursue.

Finally, questions might be raised regarding the “self-generated” component of VW-Foresight because a relatively obvious solution is suggested by the problem. While this aspect of the design certainly reduces the degree of difficulty associated with the self-directed problem identification and intention formation aspects of episodic foresight, in daily life it is easy to envisage many occasions where the problem and its solution might seem relatively obvious, but still require the engagement of this skill to secure benefits and avoid problems. Moreover, what was considered critical in the initial development and test validation of VW-Foresight was establishing that there was a relatively obvious problem, solution and resolution – so that where failures to acquire or use an item occurred, it could be readily attributed to a specific difficulty engaging in episodic foresight, and not with broader problem-solving abilities (for a further discussion of these issues, see Lyons et al., Reference Lyons, Henry, Rendell, Corballis and Suddendorf2014). Importantly, and as noted previously, VW-Foresight was designed to meet Suddendorf and Corballis’ (Reference Suddendorf and Corballis2010) stringent experimental design criteria for assessing episodic foresight. Nevertheless, it should be acknowledged that because of the relatively obvious nature of the problems and solutions presented, VW-Foresight should be regarded as measuring the capacity to flexibly exercise a quite basic level of episodic foresight only.

In conclusion, because of the considerable overlap in the neural structures critical for episodic simulation and those disrupted in PD, there was a strong neurobiological basis for predicting PD-related impairment in this capacity. It is therefore surprising that, prior to this study, the ability to initiate and apply episodic foresight had not previously been explored in PD, or indeed any other movement disorder. In line with preregistered predictions, the results from this study provide the first direct empirical evidence that the functional aspect of episodic foresight is significantly and substantially compromised in PD. Further, findings suggest that these difficulties may be a stable symptom of PD, and one that is quite distinct from other neurocognitive and clinical disease features. More work is now required to better understand when and why these deficits occur, and how they may impact functional capacity in everyday life. Such research will directly inform the development of interventions aimed at supporting and improving ones’ ability to engage in self-generated future behavioral intentions, and thus allow people with PD to maintain a higher level of independence.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617722000182

Acknowledgements

We would like to extend a thank you to our participants for donating their time, as well as their loved ones for completing the informant measures, Dr. Daniel Schweitzer and Liisa Laakso for provision of practical support, and the entire Wynnum movement clinic team, as this study would not have been possible without their continued assistance.

Funding statement

J. D. H. was supported by an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship (FT170100096).

Conflicts of interest

None.

Footnotes

1 Due to COVID restrictions, The PD and healthy comparison group were recruited and tested concurrently. Early checks of demographic matching revealed many group differences in key demographics. Accordingly, prior to running any of the study’s analyses, 18 participants were removed from the comparison group. These exclusions were based solely on their demographic characteristics. Ten new neurotypical participants (with the required demographic characteristics) were then tested as their replacements.

2 As results were unaltered with these participants’ exclusion, all participants were retained in the analyses.

References

Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Constructive episodic simulation: temporal distance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement. Hippocampus, 18, 227237. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20405 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altgassen, M., Rendell, P. G., Bernhard, A., Henry, J. D., Bailey, P. E., Phillips, L. H., & Kliegel, M. (2015). Future thinking improves prospective memory and plan enactment in older adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 192204. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.956127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2001). Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 533539. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benoit, R. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertossi, E., Tesini, C., Cappelli, A., & Ciaramelli, E. (2016). Ventromedial prefrontal damage causes a pervasive impairment of episodic memory and future thinking. Neuropsychologia, 90, 1224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandt, J., & Benedict, R. H. (2001). Hopkins verbal learning test – revised: professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Brück, A., Kurki, T., Kaasinen, V., Vahlberg, T., & Rinne, J. O. (2004). Hippocampal and prefrontal atrophy in patients with early non-demented Parkinson’s disease is related to cognitive impairment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 75, 14671469. https://jnnp.bmj.com/ CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camicioli, R. M., Korzan, J. R., Foster, S. L., Emery, D. J., Bastos, A. C., & Hanstock, C. C. (2004). Posterior cingulate metabolic changes occur in Parkinson’s disease patients without dementia. Neuroscience Letters, 354, 177180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2003.09.076 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapuis, S., Ouchchane, L., Metz, O., Gerbaud, L., & Durif, F. (2005). Impact of the motor complications of Parkinson’s disease on the quality of life. Movement Disorders, 20, 224230. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20279 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Peppe, A., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2008). Prospective memory impairment in individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 22, 283292. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.283 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coundouris, S. P., Terrett, G., Laakso, L., Schweitzer, D., Kneebone, A., Rendell, P. G., & Henry, J. D. (2020). A meta-analytic review of prospection deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 108, 3447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2005). Assessment of executive dysfunction. In Halligan, P. W. & Wade, D. T. (Eds.), Effectiveness of rehabilitation for cognitive deficits (pp. 233245). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Vito, S., Gamboz, N., Brandimonte, M. A., Barone, P., Amboni, M., & Della Sala, S. (2012). Future thinking in Parkinson’s disease: an executive function? Neuropsychologia, 50, 14941501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorsey, E. R., Elbaz, A., Nichols, E., Abbasi, N., Abd-Allah, F., Adsuar, J. C., Abdelalim, A., Ansha, M. G, Brayne, C., Choi, J. Y. J., Collado-Mateo, D., Dahodwala, N., Do, H. P., Edessa, D., Endres, M., Fereshtehnejad, S. M., Foreman, K. J., Gankpe, F. G., Gupta, R., … Murray, C. J. L. (2018). Global, regional, and national burden of Parkinson’s disease, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurology, 17, 939953. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30295-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198. https://www.academia.edu/ CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamble, B., Moreau, D., Tippett, L. J., & Addis, D. R. (2019). Specificity of future thinking in depression: a meta-analysis. Perspectives of Psychological Sciences, 14, 816834. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619851784 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallford, D. J., Austin, D. W., Takano, K., & Raes, F. (2018). Psychopathology and episodic future thinking: a systematic review and meta-analysis of specificity and episodic detail. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 102, 4251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.01.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hariz, G. M., & Forsgren, L. (2011). Activities of daily living and quality of life in persons with newly diagnosed Parkinson’s disease according to subtype of disease, and in comparison to healthy controls. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 123, 2027. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.2010.01344.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrell, F. E. (2021). Hmisc: Harrell miscellaneous. R package version 4.5-0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmiscggpubr Google Scholar
Henry, J. D. (2021). Prospective memory impairment in neurological disorders: implications and management. Nature Reviews Neurology, 17, 297307. https://www.nature.com/ CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 608622. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704104141 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassambara, A. (2020). ggpubr: ‘ggplot2’ based publication ready plots. R package version 0.4.0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggpubr Google Scholar
Kassambara, A. (2021). rstatix: pipe-friendly framework for basic statistical tests. R package version 0.7.0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rstatix Google Scholar
Kaufer, D. I., Cummings, J. L., Ketchel, P., Smith, V., MacMillan, A., Shelley, T., Lopez, O. L., & DeKosky, S.T. (2000). Validation of the NPI-Q, a brief clinical form of the neuropsychiatric inventory. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 12, 233239. https://neuro.psychiatryonline.org/ CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Plan formation, retention, and execution in prospective memory: a new approach and age-related effects. Memory & Cognition, 28, 10411049. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209352 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klobušiaková, P., Mareček, R., Fousek, J., Výtvarová, E., & Rektorová, I. (2019). Connectivity between brain networks dynamically reflects cognitive status of Parkinson’s disease: a longitudinal study. Journel of Alzheimer’s Disease, 67, 971984. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-180834 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koerts, J., König, M., Tucha, L., & Tucha, O. (2016). Working capacity of patients with Parkinson’s disease – a systematic review. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 27, 924. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.03.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudlicka, A., Clare, L., & Hindle, J. V. (2011). Executive functions in Parkinson’s disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. Movement Disorders, 26, 23052315. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.23868 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudlicka, A., Hindle, J. V., Spencer, L. E., & Clare, L. (2018). Everyday functioning of people with Parkinson’s disease and impairments in executive function: a qualitative investigation. Disability and Rehabilitation, 40, 23512363. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2017.1334240 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, A., Henry, J. D., Robinson, G., Rendell, P. G., & Suddendorf, T. (2019). Episodic foresight and stroke. Neuropsychology, 33, 93102. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000495 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, A. D., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., Corballis, M. C., & Suddendorf, T. (2014). Episodic foresight and aging. Psychology and Aging, 29, 873884. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038130 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, A. D., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., Robinson, G., & Suddendorf, T. (2016). Episodic foresight and schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 107122. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12092 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macleod, A. D., Taylor, K. S., & Counsell, C. E. (2014). Mortality in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Movement Disorders, 29, 16151622. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.25898 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marin, R. S., Biedrzycki, R. C., & Firinciogullari, S. (1991). Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psychiatry Research, 38, 143162. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(91)90040-v CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maserejian, N., Vinikoor-Imler, L., & Dilley, A. (2020). Estimation of the 2020 global population of Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 35, 198. https://www.mdsabstracts.org/ Google Scholar
Mercuri, K., Terrett, G., Henry, J. D., Curran, H. V., Elliott, M., & Rendell, P. G. (2018). Episodic foresight deficits in regular, but not recreational, cannabis users. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32, 876882. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881118776672 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miloyan, B., McFarlane, K. A., & Suddendorf, T. (2019). Measuring mental time travel: is the hippocampus really critical for episodic memory and episodic foresight? Cortex, 117, 371384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.01.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H. E., & Willison, J. (1991). National adult reading test (NART). NFER-Nelson Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Niethammer, M., Feigin, A., & Eidelberg, D. (2012). Functional neuroimaging in Parkinson’s disease. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 2, e009274. https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a009274 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelizzari, L., Di Tella, S., Rossetto, F., Laganà, M. M., Bergsland, N., Piratstru, A., Meloni, M., Nemni, R., & Baglio, F. et al. (2020). Parietal perfusion alterations in Parkinson’s disease patients without dementia. Frontiers in Neurology, 11, 562. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2020.00562 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peto, V., Jenkinson, C., Fitzpatrick, R., & Greenhall, R. (1995). The development and validation of a short measure of functioning and well-being for individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Quality of Life Research, 4, 241248. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02260863 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., & Levin, H. S. (1997). The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and Language, 57, 394405. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1755 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The ghosts of past and future: a memory that works by piecing together bits of the past may be better suited to simulating future events than one that is a store of perfect records. Nature, 445, 27. https://doi.org/10.1038/445027a CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 657661. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic simulation of future events: concepts, data, and applications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 3960. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., & Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic future thinking: mechanisms and functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 4150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. J., Souchay, C., & Moulin, C. J. (2011). Metamemory and prospective memory in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 25, 734740. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025475 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreng, R. N., & Levine, B. (2006). The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan. Memory & Cognition, 34, 16441651. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195927 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133167. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ Google ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299313. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07001975 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2010). Behavioural evidence for mental time travel in nonhuman animals. Behavioral Brain Research, 215, 292298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.11.044 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T., & Henry, J. D. (2013). Proximate and ultimate perspectives on memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2, 246247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2013.10.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Moore, C. (2011). Introduction to the special issue: the development of episodic foresight. Cognitive Development, 26, 295298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.09.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrett, G., Lyons, A., Henry, J. D., Ryrie, C., Suddendorf, T., & Rendell, P. G. (2017). Acting with the future in mind is impaired in long-term opiate users. Psychopharmacology, 234, 99108. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4442-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terrett, G., Rose, N. S., Henry, J. D., Bailey, P. E., Altgassen, M., Phillips, L. H., Kliegel, M., & Rendell, P. G. (2016). The relationship between prospective memory and episodic future thinking in younger and older adulthood. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 310323. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1054294 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trenkwalder, C., Kohnen, R., Högl, B., Metta, V., Sixel-Döring, F., Frauscher, B., Hülsmann, J., Martinez-Martin, P., & Chaudhuri, K. R. (2011). Parkinson’s disease sleep scale: validation of the revised version PDSS-2. Movement Disorders, 26, 644652. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.23476 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, G. S., & Leverenz, J. B. (2010). Profile of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Pathology, 20, 640645. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-3639.2010.00373.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale – (WAIS–IV). NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D. A., François, R., Grolemund, G., Hayes, A, Henry, L., Hester, J., Kuhn, M., Pedersen, T. L., Miller, E., Bache, S. M., Müller, K., Ooms, J., Robinson, D., Seidel, D. P., Spinu, V., … Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4, e1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wressle, E., Engstrand, C., & Granérus, A. K. (2007). Living with Parkinson’s disease: elderly patients’ and relatives’ perspective on daily living. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 54, 131139. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1630.2006.00610.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, Y., Chen, Z., Zhang, R., Xu, T., & Feng, T. (2020). Neural substrates underlying episodic future thinking: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neuropsychologia, 138, e107255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107255 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 67, 361370. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Parkinson’s disease related participant information

Figure 1

Figure 1. Virtual week-foresight task breakdown.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Performance on virtual week-foresight for Parkinson’s disease and comparison group participants. Bars represent one standard error of the mean.

Figure 3

Table 2. Pearson’s bivariate correlations of background cognitive and clinical measures with virtual week-foresight (unconditional item use)

Supplementary material: File

Coundouris et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Coundouris et al. supplementary material(File)
File 27.2 KB