Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-fv566 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T22:06:55.878Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Implicit learning of non-spatial sequences in schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2005

CHERIE L. MARVEL
Affiliation:
Mental Health Service Line, Washington DC Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Washington, DC Interdisciplinary Program in Neuroscience, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC Department of Psychiatry, Kober-Cogan Building, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Department of Neurology, 1620 McElderry Street, Reed Hall East-2, Baltimore, MD, 21205. E-mail: cmarvel1@jhmi.edu
BARBARA L. SCHWARTZ
Affiliation:
Mental Health Service Line, Washington DC Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Washington, DC Interdisciplinary Program in Neuroscience, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC Department of Psychiatry, Kober-Cogan Building, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC
DARLENE V. HOWARD
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Program in Neuroscience, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC
JAMES H. HOWARD
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Catholic University of America, Washington, DC Department of Neurology, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC

Abstract

Recent studies have reported abnormal implicit learning of sequential patterns in patients with schizophrenia. Because these studies were based on visuospatial cues, the question remained whether patients were impaired simply due to the demands of spatial processing. This study examined implicit sequence learning in 24 patients with schizophrenia and 24 healthy controls using a non-spatial variation of the serial reaction time test (SRT) in which pattern stimuli alternated with random stimuli on every other trial. Both groups showed learning by responding faster and more accurately to pattern trials than to random trials. Patients, however, showed a smaller magnitude of sequence learning. Both groups were unable to demonstrate explicit knowledge of the nature of the pattern, confirming that learning occurred without awareness. Clinical variables were not correlated with the patients' learning deficits. Patients with schizophrenia have a decreased ability to develop sensitivity to regularly occurring sequences of events within their environment. This type of deficit may affect an array of cognitive and motor functions that rely on the perception of event regularity. (JINS, 2005, 11, 659–667.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S., MacDonald, A.W., 3rd, Braver, T.S., & Cohen, J.D. (2003). Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: Diagnostic specificity, 4-week course, and relationships to clinical symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 132143.Google Scholar
Blair, J.R. & Spreen, O. (1989). Predicting premorbid I.Q.: A revision of the National Adult Reading Test. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 43, 129136.Google Scholar
Braff, D.L. (1993). Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophrenic Bulletin, 19, 233259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J.D. & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1992). Context, cortex, and dopamine: A connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review, 99, 4577.Google Scholar
Curran, T. (1997). Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge. Psychological Research, 60, 2441.Google Scholar
Dennis, N.A., Howard, J.H., Jr., & Howard, D.V. (2003). Age deficits in learning sequences of spoken words. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Science Social Science, 58, P224227.Google Scholar
Elvevag, B., Egan, M.F., & Goldberg, T.E. (2000). Memory for temporal order in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 46, 187193.Google Scholar
Feeney, J.J., Howard, J.H., Jr., & Howard, D.V. (2002). Implicit learning of higher order sequences in middle age. Psychology and Aging, 17, 351355.Google Scholar
First, M., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. (1997). User's guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders–Clinical version (SCID–IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Gras-Vincendon, A., Danion, J.-M., Grange, D., Bilik, M., Willard-Schroeder, D., Sichel, J.-P., & Singer, L. (1994). Explicit memory, repetition priming and cognitive skill learning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 13, 117126.Google Scholar
Green, M., Kern, R., Williams, O., McGurk, S., & Kee, K. (1997). Procedural learning in schizophrenia: Evidence from serial reaction time. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 123134.Google Scholar
Howard, D.V. & Howard, J.H., Jr. (2001). When it does hurt to try: adult age differences in the effects of instructions on implicit pattern learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 798805.Google Scholar
Howard, D.V., Howard, J.H., Jr., Japikse, K.C., DiYanni, C., Thompson, A., & Somberg, R. (2004). Implicit sequence learning: Effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice. Psychology and Aging, 19, 7992.Google Scholar
Howard, J.H., Jr. & Howard, D.V. (1997). Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychology and Aging, 12, 634656.Google Scholar
Howard, J.H., Jr., Howard, D.V., Dennis, N.A., Yankovich, H., & Vaidya, C.J. (2004). Implicit spatial contextual learning in healthy aging. Neuropsychology, 18, 124134.Google Scholar
Japikse, K.C., Howard, D.V., & Howard, J.H., Jr. (2001). Evaluation of a direct nonverbal measure of declarative sequence knowledge [Abstract]. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (Suppl. 13), 62.Google Scholar
Japikse, K.C., Negash, S., Howard, J.H., Jr., & Howard, D.V. (2003). Intermanual transfer of procedural learning after extended practice of probabilistic sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 3849.Google Scholar
Kay, S., Opler, L., & Fiszbein, A. (1986). Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
Kumari, V., Gray, J.A., Honey, G.D., Soni, W., Bullmore, E.T., Williams, S.C., Ng, V.W., Vythelingum, G.N., Simmons, A., Suckling, J., Corr, P.J., & Sharma, T. (2002). Procedural learning in schizophrenia: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Schizophrenia Research, 57, 97107.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M.D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109137.Google Scholar
Manschreck, T.C., Maher, B.A., Candela, S.F., Redmond, D., Yurgelun-Todd, D., & Tsuang, M. (2000). Impaired verbal memory is associated with impaired motor performance in schizophrenia: Relationship to brain structure. Schizophrenia Research, 43, 2132.Google Scholar
Marvel, C.L., Schwartz, B.L., & Rosse, R.B. (2004). A quantitative measure of postural sway deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 68, 363372.Google Scholar
Negash, S., Howard, D.V., Japikse, K.C., & Howard, J.H., Jr. (2003). Age-related differences in implicit learning of non-spatial sequential patterns. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 10, 108121.Google Scholar
NIMH. (1976). Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health, Education and Welfare.
Nissen, M.J. & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 132.Google Scholar
Nissen, M.J., Willingham, D., & Hartman, M. (1989). Explicit and implicit remembering: When is learning preserved in amnesia? Neuropsychologia, 27, 341352.Google Scholar
Park, S. & Holzman, P.S. (1992). Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 975982.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.S., & Hallet, M. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson's disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annals of Neurology, 34, 594602.Google Scholar
Phillips, M.L. & David, A.S. (1997). Viewing strategies for simple and chimeric faces: An investigation of perceptual bias in normals and schizophrenic patients using visual scan paths. Brain Cognition, 35, 225238.Google Scholar
Pinkham, A.E., Penn, D.L., Perkins, D.O., & Lieberman, J. (2003). Implications for the neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 815824.Google Scholar
Posada, A., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., & Jeannerod, M. (2001). Anticipating incoming events: An impaired cognitive process in schizophrenia. Cognition, 81, 209225.Google Scholar
Quamme, J.R., Yonelinas, A.P., Widaman, K.F., Kroll, N.E., & Sauve, M.J. (2004). Recall and recognition in mild hypoxia: Using covariance structural modeling to test competing theories of explicit memory. Neuropsychologia, 42, 672691.Google Scholar
Reber, P.J. & Squire, L.R. (1998). Encapsulation of implicit and explicit memory in sequence learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 248263.Google Scholar
Remillard, G. & Clark, J.M. (2001). Implicit learning of first-, second-, and third-order transition probabilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 483498.Google Scholar
Schvaneveldt, R. & Gomez, R. (1998). Attention and probabilistic sequence learning. Psychological Research, 61, 175190.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B.L., Howard, D.V., Howard, J.H., Jr., Hovaguimian, A., & Deutsch, S.I. (2003). Implicit learning of visuospatial sequences in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 17, 517533.Google Scholar
Sullivan, E.V., Fama, R., Shear, P.K., Cahn-Weiner, D.A., Stein, M., Zipursky, R.B., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2001). Motor sequencing deficits in schizophrenia: A comparison with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology, 15, 342350.Google Scholar
Sullivan, E.V., Rosenbloom, M.J., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2004). Balance and gait deficits in schizophrenia compounded by the comorbidity of alcoholism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 751755.Google Scholar