Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xfwgj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-16T22:50:43.015Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hemispheric antagonism in visuo-spatial neglect: A case study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

John C. Marshall
Affiliation:
University Department of Clinical Neurology, The Radeliffe Infirmary, Oxford, U.K.
Peter W. Halligan
Affiliation:
Rivermead Rehabilitation Centre, Oxford, U.K.

Abstract

We report a case of severe visuo-spatial neglect consequent upon right-hemisphere stroke. At the time of testing, the patient had no visual field cut and no significant hemiparesis. Conventional testing on cancellation tasks with the right hand revealed reliable left neglect, but performance was significantly improved when the left hand was used. Investigations of (manual) line bisection showed normal performance with the right hand but right neglect when the left hand was used. Right neglect was also observed on a purely perceptual version of the line bisection task. We argue that the attentional vectors of the cerebral hemispheres can be modulated by (perceptual) task-demands and by (motorie) response demands. (JINS, 1996, 2, 412–418.)

Type
Thematic Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Binder, J., Marshall, R., Lazar, R., Benjamin, J., & Mohr, J.P. (1992). Distinct syndromes of hemineglect. Archives of Neurology, 49, 11871194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, H.D. & Kosslyn, S.M. (1993). Cerebral lateralization. Current Opinion in Neurobiolog, 3, 183186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, A. & Warrington, E.K. (1987). The dissociation of vi-suospatial neglect and neglect dyslexia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 50, 11101116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cubelli, R., Niehelli, P., Bonito, V., DeTanti, A., & Inzaghi, M.G. (1991). Different patterns of dissociation in unilateral neglect. Brain and Cognition, 15, 139159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daffner, K.R., Ahern, G.L., Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M.M. (1990). Dissociated neglect behavior following sequential strokes in the right hemisphere. Annals of Neurology, 28, 97101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. (1986). Neglect in a patient with partial callosal disconnection. Neuropsychologia, 24, 397403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halligan, P.W., Cockburn, J., & Wilson, B. (1991a). The behavioural assessment of visual neglect. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 1, 532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halligan, P.W., Manning, L., & Marshall, J.C. (1991b). Hemispheric activation vs spatio-motor cueing in visual neglect: A case study. Neuropsychologia, 29, 165175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halligan, P.W. & Marshall, J.C. (1989). Laterality of motor response in visuo-spatial neglect: A case study. Neuropsychologia, 27, 13011307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halligan, P.W. & Marshall, J.C. (1992). Left visuo-spatial neglect: A meaningless entity? Cortex, 28, 525535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halligan, P.W. & Marshall, J.C. (1994a). Toward a principled explanation of unilateral neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 167206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halligan, P.W. & Marshall, J.C. (1994b). Focal and global attention modulate the expression of visuo-spatial neglect: A case study. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heilman, K.M., Bowers, D., & Watson, R.T. (1984). Pseudo-neglect in a patient with partial callosal disconnection. Brain, 107, 519532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G.W. & Riddoch, M.J. (1994). Attention to within-object and between-object representations: Multiple sites for visual attention. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 207241.Google Scholar
Humphreys, G.W. & Riddoch, M.J. (1995). Separate coding of space within and between perceptual objects: Evidence from unilateral visual neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 283311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irving-Bell, L. (1993). Neurophysiology and neuropsychology of visuo-spatial neglect: Analysis of spatial cues in normal control subjects and patients with neglect. Honours project, University of Oxford.Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1993). Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In Robertson, I.H. & Marshall, J.C. (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 6386). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1994). Lateral interactions in the brain. In Kins-bourne, M. & Smith, W. Lynn (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S.M., Chabris, C.F., Marsolek, C.J., & Koenig, O. (1992). Categorical versus co-ordinate spatial relations: Computational analyses and computer simulations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 562577.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S.M., Koenig, O., Barrett, A., Cave, C.B., Tang, J., & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (1989). Evidence for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for categorical and co-ordinate relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 723735.Google Scholar
Kwon, S.E. & Heilman, K.M. (1991). Ipsilateral neglect in a patient following a unilateral frontal lesion. Neurology, 41, 20012004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lui, G.T., Bolton, A.K., Price, B.H., & Weintraub, S. (1992). Dis-sociated perceptuo-sensory and exploratory-motor neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 55, 701706.Google Scholar
Marshall, R.S., Lazar, R.L., Van Heertum, R.L., Esser, P.D., Nour, R., & Mohr, J.P. (1994). A SPECT activation study of visuo-spatial functions in normals. Neurology, 44 (suppl. 2), A293.Google Scholar
Newcombe, F., Oldfield, R.C., & Wingfield, A. (1964). Object naming by dysphasic patients. Nature, 207, 12171218.Google Scholar
Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handed-ness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., Kinsbourne, M., & Moscovitch, M. (1990). Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain and Cognition, 12, 240266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter-Lorenz, P.A. & Posner, M.I. (1990). Components of neglect from right-hemisphere damage: An analysis of line bisection. Neuropsychologia, 28, 327333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riddoch, M.J., Humphreys, G.W., Luckhurst, L., Burroughs, E., & Bateman, A. (1995). “Paradoxical neglect”: Spatial representations, hemisphere-specific activation, and spatial cueing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 569604.Google Scholar
Robertson, I.H., Halligan, P.W., Bergego, C., Homberg, V., Pizzamiglio, L., Weber, E., & Wilson, B.A. (1994). Right neglect following right hemisphere damage. Cortex, 30, 199213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, I.H. & North, N. (1992). Spatio-motor cueing in unilateral left neglect: The role of hemispace, hand and motor activation. Neuropsychologia, 30, 553563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarisbrick, D.J., Tweedy, J.R., & Kuslansky, G. (1987). Hand preference and performance effects on line bisection. Neuropsychologia, 25, 695699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallar, G. (1994). Left hemineglect: An unmanageable explosion of dissociations? No. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4, 209212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar