Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T23:49:37.154Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Change in Cognitive Abilities in Older Latinos

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2015

Robert S. Wilson*
Affiliation:
Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurological Sciences, Department of Behavioral Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
Ana W. Capuano
Affiliation:
Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
David X. Marquez
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Chicago, Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition, Chicago, Illinois
Priscilla Amofa
Affiliation:
Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
Lisa L. Barnes
Affiliation:
Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurological Sciences, Department of Behavioral Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
David A. Bennett
Affiliation:
Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Robert S. Wilson, Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, 600 South Paulina Street, Suite 1027a, Chicago, IL, 60612. E-mail: rwilson@rush.edu

Abstract

The aim of this study was to compare patterns of cognitive decline in older Latinos and non-Latinos. At annual intervals for a mean of 5.7 years, older Latino (n=104) and non-Latino (n=104) persons of equivalent age, education, and race completed a battery of 17 cognitive tests from which previously established composite measures of episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, perceptual speed, and visuospatial ability were derived. In analyses adjusted for age, sex, and education, performance declined over time in each cognitive domain, but there were no ethnic group differences in initial level of function or annual rate of decline. There was evidence of retest learning following the baseline evaluation, but neither the magnitude nor duration of the effect was related to Latino ethnicity, and eliminating the first two evaluations, during which much of retest learning occurred, did not affect ethnic group comparisons. Compared to the non-Latino group, the Latino group had more diabetes (38.5% vs. 25.0; χ2[1]=4.4; p=.037), fewer histories of smoking (24.0% vs. 39.4%, χ2[1]=5.7; p=.017), and lower childhood household socioeconomic level (−0.410 vs. −0.045, t[185.0]=3.1; p=.002), but controlling for these factors did not affect results. Trajectories of cognitive aging in different abilities are similar in Latino and non-Latino individuals of equivalent age, education, and race. (JINS, 2016, 22, 58–65)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Albert, M., Smith, L.A., Scherr, P.A., Taylor, J.O., Evans, D.A., & Funkenstein, H.H. (1991). Use of brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Neuroscience, 57, 167178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alley, D., Suthers, K., & Crimmins, E. (2007). Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: Results from the AHEAD sample. Research on Aging, 29, 7394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arvanitakis, Z., Bennett, D.A., Wilson, R.S., & Barnes, L.L. (2010). Diabetes and cognitive systems in older Black and White persons. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Disorders, 24, 3742.Google Scholar
Barnes, L.L., Shah, R.C., Aggarwal, N.T., Bennett, D.A., & Schneider, J.A. (2012). The Minority Aging Research Study: Ongoing efforts to obtain brain donation in African Americans without dementia. Current Alzheimer’s Research, 9, 736747.Google Scholar
Bartels, C., Wegrzyn, M., Wiedl, A., Ackermann, V., & Ehrenreich, H. (2010). Practice effects in healthy adults: A longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing. BMC Neuroscience, 11, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D.A., Schneider, J.A., Buchman, A.S., Mendes de Leon, C.F., & Wilson, R.S. (2005). The Rush Memory and Aging Project: Study design and baseline characteristics of the study cohort. Neuroepidemiology, 25, 163175.Google Scholar
Bennett, D.A., Schneider, J.A., Arvanitakis, Z., & Wilson, R.S. (2012). Overview and findings from the Religious Orders Study. Current Alzheimer Research, 9, 628645.Google Scholar
Bennett, D.A., Schneider, J.A., Buchman, A.S., Barnes, L.L., Boyle, P.A., & Wilson, R.S. (2012). Overview and findings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Current Alzheimer Research, 9, 646663.Google Scholar
Benton, A.L., Sivan, A.B., Hamsher, K. deS, Varney, N.R., & Spreen, O. (1994). Contributions to neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Collins, N., Sachs-Ericsson, N., Preacher, K.J., Sheffield, K.M., & Markides, K. (2009). Smoking increases risk for cognitive decline among community-dwelling older Mexican Americans. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 934942.Google Scholar
Early, D.R., Widaman, K.F., Harvey, D., Beckett, L., Park, L.Q., Farias, S.T., & Mungas, D. (2013). Demographic predictors of cognitive decline in ethnically diverse older persons. Psychology and Aging, 28, 633645.Google Scholar
Ekstrom, R.B., French, J.W., Harman, H.H., & Kermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
Gellman, A., Carli, J.B., Stern, H.S., & Rubin, D.B. (2004). Bayesian data analysis. New York, NY: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Hildreth, K.L., Grigsby, J., Bryant, L.L., Wolfe, P., & Baxter, J. (2014). Cognitive decline and cardiometabolic risk among Hispanic and non-Hispanic white adults in the San Luis Valley Health and Aging Study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 332342.Google Scholar
Hoffman, L., Hofer, S.M., & Silwinski, M.J. (2011). On the confounds among retest gains and age-cohort differences in the estimation of within-person change in longitudinal studies: A simulation study. Psychology and Aging, 26, 778791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, E.F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Karlamangla, A.S., Miller-Martinez, D., Aneshensel, C.S., Seeman, T.E., Wight, R.G., & Chodosh, J. (2009). Trajectories of cognitive function in late life in the United States: Demographic and socioeconomic predictors. American Journal of Epidemiology, 170, 331342.Google Scholar
Krueger, K.R., Wilson, R.S., Bennett, D.A., & Aggarwal, N.T. (2009). A battery of tests for assessing cognitive function in older Latino persons. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 23, 384388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, N., & Ware, J. (1982). Random effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics, 38, 963974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lunn, D., Spiegelhalther, D., Thomas, A., & Best, N. (2009). The BUGS project: Evolution, critique and future directions (with discussion). Statistics in Medicine, 28, 30493082.Google Scholar
Masel, M.C., & Peek, M.K. (2009). Ethnic differences in cognitive function over time. Annals of Epidemiology, 19, 778783.Google Scholar
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rassen, J.A., Shelat, A.A., Myers, J., Glynn, R.J., Rothman, K.J., & Schneeweiss, S. (2012). One-to-many propensity score matching in cohort studies. Pharmacoepidemiology and Drug Safety, 21, 6980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J.C., Court, J.H., & Raven, J. (1992). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary: Standard progressive matrices. Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Schleicher, D.J., Van Iddekinge, C.H., Morgeson, F.P., & Campion, M.A. (2010). If at first you don’t succeed, try, try again: Understanding race, age, and gender differences in retesting score improvement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 603617.Google Scholar
Singer, J.D., & Willett, J.B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sliwinski, M.J., Stawski, R.S., Hall, C.B., Katz, M., Verghese, J., & Lipton, R. (2006). Distinguishing preterminal and terminal cognitive decline. European Psychologist, 11, 172181.Google Scholar
Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test manual-revised. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Van Iddekinge, C.H., Morgeson, F.P., Schleicher, D.J., & Campion, M.A. (2011). Can I retake it? Exploring subgroup differences and criterion-related validity in promotion retesting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 941955.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Welsh, K.A., Butters, N., Mohs, R.C., Beekly, D., Edland, S., Fillenbaum, G., & Heyman, A. (1994). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), part V: A normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology, 44, 609614.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Aggarwal, N.T., Barnes, L.L., Bienias, J.L., Mendes de Leon, C.F., & Evans, D.A. (2009). Biracial population study of mortality in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 66, 767772.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Barnes, L.L., & Bennett, D.A. (2003). Assessment of lifetime participation in cognitively stimulating activities. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 634642.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Barnes, L.L., Krueger, K.R., Hoganson, G., Bienias, J.L., & Bennett, D.A. (2005). Early and late life cognitive activity and cognitive systems in old age. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 400407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R.S., Beckett, L.A., Barnes, L.L., Schneider, J.A., Bach, J., Evans, D.A., & Bennett, D.A. (2002). Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychology and Aging, 17, 179193.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Beckett, L.A., Bienias, J.L., Evans, D.A., & Bennett, D.A. (2003). Terminal decline in cognitive function. Neurology, 60, 17821787.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Bienias, J.L., Evans, D.A., & Bennett, D.A. (2004). Religious Orders Study: Overview and change in cognitive and motor speed. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 11, 280303.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Capuano, A.W., Sytsma, J., Bennett, D.A., & Barnes, L.L. (2015). Cognitive aging in older Black and White persons. Psychology and Aging, 30, 279285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R.S., Li, Y., Bienias, J.L., & Bennett, D.A. (2006). Cognitive decline in old age: Separating retest effects from the effects of growing older. Psychology and Aging, 21, 774789.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Segawa, E., Hizel, L.P., Boyle, P.A., & Bennett, D.A. (2012). Terminal dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities. Neurology, 78, 11161122.Google Scholar
Yang, L., Reed, M., & Kuan, C. (2012). Retest learning in the absence of item-specific effects: Does it show in the oldest-old? Psychology and Aging, 27, 701706.Google Scholar
Zeki Al Hazzouri, A., Haan, M.N., Kalbfleisch, J.D., Galea, S., Lisabeth, L.D., & Aiello, A.E. (2011). Life-course socioeconomic position and incidence of dementia and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans: Results from the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging. American Journal of Epidemiology, 173, 11481158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeki Al Hazzouri, A., Haan, M.N., Neuhaus, J.M., Pletcher, M., Peralta, C.A., Lopez, L., & Stable, E.J.P. (2013). Cardiovascular risk score, cognitive decline, and dementia in older Mexican Americans: The role of sex and education. Journal of the American Heart Association, 2, e004978.Google Scholar
Zeki Al Hazzouri, A., Haan, M.N., Osypuk, T., Abdou, C., Hinton, L., & Aiello, A.E. (2011). Neighborhood socioeconomic context and cognitive decline among older Mexican Americans: Results from the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging. American Journal of Epidemiology, 174, 423431.Google Scholar