Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T05:27:44.388Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Navigation by Dead Reckoning and Local Cues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2009

A. S. Etienne
Affiliation:
(University of Geneva)
V. Séguinot
Affiliation:
(University of Geneva)

Extract

According to comprehensive theories of navigation, animals navigate by using two complementary strategies: (1) dead reckoning informs the subject in a continuous manner on its actual location with respect to an Earthbound or absolute coordinate system; while (2) long-term associations between particular landmarks and specific locations allow the animal to find its way within a familiar environment. If the subject structures familiar space as a system of interconnected places – the so-called ‘cognitive map’ – it may know through dead reckoning where it is located on its map and relate its route-based expectations to the actually perceived scenario of local cues.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal Institute of Navigation 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

1Bennett, A. T. D. (1993). A framework of spatial memory models. Paper presented at the Conference of the Royal Institute of Navigation, Oxford.Google Scholar
2Collett, T. S., Cartwright, B. A. and Smith, B. A. (1986). Landmark learning and visuospatial memories in gerbils. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 158, 835851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Etienne, A. S., Hurni, C., Maurer, R. and Séguinot, V. (1991). Twofold path integration during hoarding in the golden hamster? Ethology, Ecology and Evolution, 3, 111.Google Scholar
4Etienne, A. S., Joris, S., Maurer, R. and Teroni, E. (1990). Enhancing the impact of extramaze cues in a spatial orientation task. Behavioural Brain Research, 38, 199210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5Etienne, A. S., Maurer, R. and Saucy, F. (1988). Limitations in the assessment of pathdependent information. Behaviour, 106, 81111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6Etienne, A. S., Maurer, R., Saucy, F. and Teroni, E. (1986). Short-distance homing in the golden hamster after a passive outward journey. Animal Behaviour 34, 696715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7Etienne, A. S., Teroni, E., Hurni, C. and Portenier, V. (1990). The effect of a single light cue on homing behaviour of the golden hamster. Animal Behaviour, 39, 1741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
8Gallistel, C. R.The Organization of Learning (1990). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
9Georgakopoulos, J. and Etienne, A. S. Identifying location by dead reckoning and local cues. Behavioural Processes. (In the Press.)Google Scholar
10Maurer, R. (1993). Dead reckoning in animals: can PDP help us unravel the mystery of partial solutions? Paper presented at the Conference of the Royal Institute of Navigation, Oxford.Google Scholar
11O'Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Clarendon Press, London.Google Scholar
12Séguinot, V., Maurer, R. and Etienne, A. S. Dead reckoning in a small mammal: the evaluation of distance. Journal of Comparative Physiology. (In the Press.)Google Scholar
13Teroni, E., Portenier, V. and Etienne, A. S. (1987). Spatial orientation of the golden hamster in conditions of conflicting location-based and route-based information. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 20, 389397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar