Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-x5cpj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-30T04:45:27.199Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Productive and non-productive morphology: the case of the German strong verbs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

J. S. Barbour
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistic and International Studies, University of Surrey

Extract

In this paper various conclusions will be drawn from a comparison of productive and non-productive German verb morphology; more specifically, it examines some of the properties of the morphological component of a generative grammar of German.1

Two assumptions are made at the outset, which clearly distinguish the position adopted from that of the standard model of transformational phonology as exemplifed particularly in Chomsky & Halle (1968) (SPE). The first of these refers to the position of morphological rules in a grammar: it is that, for at least a large number of languages, the phenomena of morphology are not appropriately dealt with by syntactic, readjustment and phonological rules, in the manner of SPE, but that distinct morphological rules are required, rules which are, in particular, quite distinct from phonological rules.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, S. R. (1977). On the formal description of inflection. PCLS 13. 1544.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bierwisch, M. (1967). Skizze der generativen Phonologie. In Phonologische Studien (Studia Grammatica 6): 733. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Danell, K. J. (1978). The concept of vagueness in linguistics. Some methodological reflections of a non-specialist. Studio Neophilologica 50. 324.Google Scholar
Halle, M. (1973). Prolegomena to a theory of word formation. Lln 4. 316.Google Scholar
Helbig, G. G. & Buscha, J. (1975). Deutsche Grammatik. Leipzig: VEB Verlag Enzyklopädie.Google Scholar
Hooper, J. B. (1976). An introduction to natural generative phonology. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hudson, G. (1974). The representation of non-productive alternation. In Historical Linguistics, Volumes from the Edinburgh Conference. Amsterdam: North Holland. 203229.Google Scholar
Isačnko, A. V. (1963). Der phonologische Status des velaren Nasals im Deutschen. Z Phon 16.7784.Google Scholar
Lockwood, W. B. (1965). An informal history of the German language. Cambridge: Heffer.Google Scholar
Maher, J. P. (1970). Etymology and generative phonology in traditional lexicon. General Linguistics, II. 7193.Google Scholar
Matthews, P. H. (1972). Inflectional morphology. London: CUP.Google Scholar
Matthews, P. H. (1974) Morphology. London: CUP.Google ScholarPubMed
Moody, M. (1978). Some preliminaries to a theory of morphology. Glossa 12. 1638.Google Scholar
Moulton, W. G. (1962). The sounds of English and German. Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Pullum, G. K. (1976). The Duke of York gambit. JL 52. 83102.Google Scholar
Ringen, C. O. (1980). Uralic and Altaic vowel harmony: a problem for natural generative phonology. JL 16. 3744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vennemann, T. (1970). The German velar nasal. A case for abstract phonology. Phonetica 22. 6581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vennemann, T. (1974). Words and syllables in natural generative grammar. Chicago Linguistic Society, Papers from the Parasession on Natural Phonology. 346374.Google Scholar
Wurzel, W. U. (1970), Studien zur deutschen Lautstruktur (Studia Grammatica 8). Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.Google Scholar