Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T04:41:32.732Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Instrumental support exchanges among kin and non-kin in light of personal configurations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2021

Gaëlle Aeby*
Affiliation:
Institute of Sociological Research, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Jacques-Antoine Gauthier
Affiliation:
Life Course and Inequality Research Centre, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: gaelle.aeby@unige.ch

Abstract

As exchanges of instrumental support between kin and non-kin remain essential to buffer the impact of critical life events, we consider the characteristics of personal configurations that may enhance or hinder them. Personal configurations vary in terms of their composition and two aspects of their structure: density and centrality. These dimensions are investigated to uncover whether they influence the type of instrumental support being exchanged (financial, material, and care) and the likelihood of their being reciprocal. Drawing on a representative sample of individuals living in Switzerland, results show that overall instrumental support is present in all personal configurations irrespective of their composition, but financial support is more prevalent in configurations based on parent–adult child relationships. Furthermore, configuration structures characterized by density of emotional support are positively associated with giving care support and with reciprocity, while those characterized by density of conflict are associated with giving less instrumental support overall.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Université catholique de Louvain 2021

1. Introduction

The development of welfare policies in Switzerland as in most Western societies following World War II has decreased the need for family instrumental support by filling the unexpected “holes” in individuals' trajectories (e.g., following accidents, disease, unemployment, and maternity leave) [Kohli (Reference Kohli2007), Wall et al. (Reference Wall, Widmer, Gauthier, Česnuitytė and Gouveia2018)]. Correlatively, states have progressively taken over some tasks traditionally carried out within families (e.g., childcare, education, and elderly care). Nevertheless, exchanges of instrumental support between family members and beyond remain essential to ease family transitions, buffer the impact of critical life events, and prevent vulnerabilization [Spini et al., (Reference Spini, Bernardi and Oris2017)]. For instance, the current pandemic crisis reactivates the need for family instrumental support [Widmer et al. (Reference Widmer, de Bel, Olga Ganjour, Zufferey, Gamba, Nardone, Cattacin and Ricciardi2020)]. Extensive research has been devoted to resources such as instrumental support that are directly or indirectly embedded in personal relationships, which individuals may get access to and further invest in or draw on when needed [Wellman and Wortley (Reference Wellman and Wortley1990), Kadushin (Reference Kadushin2002), Van Der Gaag and Snijders (Reference Van Der Gaag and Snijders2005), Ryan et al. (Reference Ryan, Sales, Tilki and Siara2008)]. Instrumental support can be of various types such as financial (loans, money transfers), material (providing goods), or care (such as domestic help, transportation, caregiving services) [Swartz (Reference Swartz2009)]. It is distinguished from emotional support (such as motivation, guidance, closeness) as it responds to more specific needs [Cohen (Reference Cohen2004)].

Instrumental support is usually associated with the Western nuclear family centered on the heterosexual couples and their minor children, which has been conceptualized as an economically independent and self-reliant unit of support [Parsons (Reference Parsons and Nanda Anshen1949)]. This focus on the nuclear family has been challenged by demographic shifts (e.g., increase in the life expectancy and co-survivorship creating a larger supply of intergenerational ties) and the changes in nuclear families themselves (greater marital instability and single parenting) [Bengtson (Reference Bengtson2001), Swartz (Reference Swartz2009)]. In this context, the support potential of a wide range of relationships is brought to light, such as instrumental support stemming from intergenerational family relationships as well as from non-kin relationships. Regarding intergenerational family relationships, research shows their latent availability under different concepts such as “family national guard” [Hagestad (Reference Hagestad and Bengtson1996)], “latent kin network of support” [Riley and Riley (Reference Riley, Riley, Achenbaum and Bengtson1993)], “support bank” [Antonucci and Jackson (Reference Antonucci, Jackson and Warnes1989)], or “kin reservoir” [Coenen-Huther et al. (Reference Coenen-Huther, Kellerhals, von Allmen, Hagmann, Jeannerat and Widmer1994)]. As for non-kin relationships, research has acknowledged the role of friends and other non-related individuals in providing instrumental support [Smart (Reference Smart2010), Suanet and Antonucci (Reference Suanet and Antonucci2017)], especially when individuals do not experience a nuclear family setting [e.g., same-sex couples, see Weeks et al. (Reference Weeks, Heaphy and Donovan2001); singlehood, see Bellotti (Reference Bellotti2008); shared living arrangements, see Heath et al. (Reference Heath, Davies, Edwards and Scicluna2017)], experience a critical life event like an intimate relationship breakdown [Aeby and Heath (Reference Aeby and Heath2020)], and explore career development opportunities [McDonald and Mair (Reference McDonald and Mair2010)].

Many individuals find themselves isolated and unable to rely on family or friendship supports [McPherson et al. (Reference McPherson, Smith-Lovin and Brashears2006), Nicolaisen and Thorsen (Reference Nicolaisen and Thorsen2017)]. Individuals tend to be acquainted with alters sharing similar circumstances [McPherson et al. (Reference McPherson, Smith-Lovin and Cook2001)] and, therefore, those with lower socioeconomic backgrounds may be unable to get access to the support required when needed (especially financial). Similarly, migration contexts reduce the availability for some types of instrumental support (especially care) [Lubbers et al. (Reference Lubbers, Molina, Lerner, Brandes, Ávila and McCarty2010), Wyss and Nedelcu (Reference Wyss and Nedelcu2020)]. Besides these factors, the explanation for the lack or, availability of instrumental support may also lay on the characteristics of the web of interdependencies that individuals are embedded in. Indeed, instrumental support exchanges between two individuals do not happen in a social vacuum, but are embedded in personal configurations [Elias (Reference Elias1978, Reference Elias1983), Widmer (Reference Widmer2010)]. These personal configurations may vary in terms of their composition and structure. Composition refers to the type of ties; for instance, whether individuals develop their personal configurations around their couple, siblings, and/or their friends [Widmer et al. (Reference Widmer, Aeby and Sapin2013)]. Structure relates to the interconnectedness of personal configurations; for instance whether individuals and their configuration members are densely or loosely connected to one another [Coleman (Reference Coleman1988), Burt (Reference Burt, Guillén, Collins, England and Meyer2002)].

Moving beyond the nuclear family, this paper explores the characteristics of personal configurations that may enhance or hinder instrumental support exchanges. More precisely, we assess the impact of the composition and structure of personal configurations on instrumental support exchanges, looking at the type of instrumental support (financial, material and care) and on the likelihood of such exchanges to be reciprocal. Indeed, to understand instrumental support exchanges, one has to consider their embeddedness in a wider web of interdependencies. In the following two sections, we first explore instrumental support exchanges among kin and non-kin, and then the characteristics of personal configurations drawing on the configurational approach.

2. Instrumental support exchanges among kin and non-kin

Individuals have access to a set of relationships that are likely to vary in commitment and givenness. While relationships with family members are at the heart of instrumental support [Bonvalet and Ogg (Reference Bonvalet and Ogg2007)], friend and work-related relationships also represent a significant source of support [Allan (Reference Allan2008), McDonald and Mair (Reference McDonald and Mair2010), Conkova et al. (Reference Conkova, Fokkema and Dykstra2018)]. There is a lasting debate in most Western societies about whether there is a process of suffusion between different sets of values bringing the roles of family and friend closer together [Pahl and Spencer (Reference Pahl and Spencer2004), Allan (Reference Allan2008), Wall and Gouveia (Reference Wall and Gouveia2014)]. Some values are commonly associated with the family, such as obligation and hierarchy, and others to friends, such as choice and equality. Some authors highlight the similarities between friend and family roles, with friends playing family-like roles and family members playing friend-like roles [Pahl and Spencer (Reference Pahl and Spencer2004), Wall and Gouveia (Reference Wall and Gouveia2014)]. However, other authors suggest that friends and family retain some specialized roles and that different principles of solidarity still exist [Allan (Reference Allan2008)]. For instance, loans and money transfers are said to be more prevalent among family members and, in particular, between parents and their adult children [Bonvalet and Ogg (Reference Bonvalet and Ogg2007)], which is partially explained by the persistent norm of filial responsibility [Swartz (Reference Swartz2009)]. Correlatively, instrumental support not implying money or goods such as care support may be more evenly widespread. Finally, female family members are the most active in participating in these exchanges; they tend to provide and receive more support than men [Swartz (Reference Swartz2009)].

In case of need and in their everyday life, most individuals get help from their personal relationships, but they are also expected to provide support to their close ones. Thus, instrumental support exchanges have a tendency to be reciprocal. Reciprocity is a key principle, ordering exchanges among individuals [Molm et al. (Reference Molm, Collett and Schaefer2007), Girardin, Reference Girardin2017]. Anthropologists have studied the mechanisms of gift exchange, emphasizing the act of giving creating a social bond between giver and receiver and a moral obligation to reciprocate [Mauss (Reference Mauss2007) [1923–24]]. This creates chains of exchanges of goods and services. These are outside the formal market and governed by culturally grounded informal norms. The principle of reciprocity implies the exchange of goods and services. Reciprocity takes place over time, and often a favor is returned much later in another form. It is mostly unspoken, but normatively expected, and individuals who fail to comply may be excluded or exclude themselves from further exchanges, especially low-income individuals [Offer (Reference Offer2012)]. Other exceptions to this reciprocity principle are found within the parent-child relationships as money transfers tend to go from parents to their adult children [Bonvalet and Ogg (Reference Bonvalet and Ogg2007)].

The structure of reciprocity depends on two structural dimensions: direct (only two actors) vs. indirect (more than two) reciprocation of benefits, and unilateral versus bilateral flow of benefits. The combination of these two further translates into three forms of exchange: direct negotiated (based on an agreement), direct reciprocal, and indirect generalized [Molm et al. (Reference Molm, Collett and Schaefer2007)]. Generalized indirect exchange produces greater social solidarity than restricted direct exchange, which is related to the issue of risk management and building trust, and to the salience of conflict in relationships [Molm et al. (Reference Molm, Collett and Schaefer2007)]. In the case of personal configurations where individuals know each other, we see chain-generalized reciprocity in contrast to pure-generalized reciprocity (unknown recipients and benefactors).

3. Composition and structure of personal configurations

The composition of personal configuration reflects individuals' embeddedness in society and, thus, is closely related to life-course stages, transitions, and events [Antonucci et al. (Reference Antonucci, Fiori, Birditt, Jackey, Lerner and Overton2010), Levy and Widmer (Reference Levy and Widmer2013), Aeby et al. (Reference Aeby, Gauthier and Widmer2019)]. The configurational approach accounts for the interdependencies individuals develop over their life by participating in multiple daily interactions. The notion of configuration was first coined by Elias (Reference Elias1978, Reference Elias1983) and later adapted to the study of family and personal relationships (see Introduction chapter). Individuals have a personal configuration composed of alters they consider as important; those alters may come from the same or different sociability settings, be related or not, and know each other or not. Therefore, one may hypothesize that the instrumental support available to individuals will vary depending on the composition of their personal configurations.

The structure of personal configurations may be described as more or less interconnected. On the one hand, some configuration structures are characterized by a high density of relationships, meaning that the majority of configuration members are in direct ties with one another. When these dense relationships are supportive, the configuration produces bonding social capital that can give a sense of protection and togetherness but also of control and lack of autonomy [Coleman (Reference Coleman1988), Burt (Reference Burt, Guillén, Collins, England and Meyer2002), Widmer (Reference Widmer2010)]. When, in contrast, these dense relationships are conflictual, it means that individuals are embedded in a very tense environment in which all configuration members tend to antagonize each other on a regular basis. On the other hand, configuration structures can be characterized by a high centrality of respondents, meaning that they mediate most relationships between configuration members. If those relationships are supportive, the configuration produces bridging social capital [Coleman (Reference Coleman1988), Burt (Reference Burt, Guillén, Collins, England and Meyer2002), Widmer (Reference Widmer2010)]. In this case, individuals who serve as a bridge between configuration members benefit from their central position to have power over flows of resources [Cook and Emerson (Reference Cook and Emerson1978), Burt (Reference Burt, Guillén, Collins, England and Meyer2002), Cornwell (Reference Cornwell2011)]. However, in conflictual relationships, the respondent is at the center of tensions [Widmer (Reference Widmer2010), Widmer et al. (Reference Widmer, Girardin and Ludwig2018)]. Frequently, supportive and conflictual relationships co-exist in the same personal configurations, and this generates triadic ambivalence [Widmer and Lüscher (Reference Widmer and Lüscher2011), Sapin et al. (Reference Sapin, Widmer and Iglesias2016), Aeby et al. (Reference Aeby, Gauthier and Widmer2020)]. While kin and non-kin may provide support, the level of co-dependence between family members is overall higher, and this explains the higher level of ambivalence in relationships with partners, parents, offspring, and siblings in comparison with those with extended family members, friends, and acquaintances [Lüscher and Pillemer (Reference Lüscher and Pillemer1998), Fingerman et al. (Reference Fingerman, Hay and Birditt2004)]. Interestingly, even in times of financial struggle, being connected can play an efficient buffering role and contribute to subjective well-being, whereas the satisfaction of the socially isolated can “catch up” to some degree if they are doing well financially [Richards (Reference Richards2016)]. Therefore, one may hypothesize that instrumental support available to individuals varies depending on the structure of their personal configurations.

4. Summary and hypotheses

Instrumental support exchanges between two individuals are embedded in personal configurations. In this study, we explore the characteristics of personal configurations that may enhance or hinder those exchanges. Personal configurations distinguish themselves from one another according to their composition and two aspects of their structure (density and centrality). These dimensions are investigated to uncover whether they influence the type of instrumental support being exchanged (financial, material, and care) and the likelihood of such exchanges being reciprocal. More precisely, considering the type of instrumental support, we propose the following hypotheses:

  1. (1a) As instrumental support exchanges arise in situations of manifest and specific needs, exchanges of care support are more frequent than financial or material support exchanges.

  2. (1b) As loans or money transfers are more common between parents and adult children, personal configurations based on close family members are more likely than personal configurations based on non-kin to provide or be given financial or material support.

  3. (1c) As configuration structures characterized by density of emotional support create a more protective environment, they are associated with more instrumental support exchanges overall, whereas configuration structures characterized by density of conflict are associated with less instrumental support exchanges overall.

Considering reciprocity, our hypotheses are:

  1. (2a) As reciprocity is an overarching principle, ordering exchanges among individuals, it is more common than unidirectional exchanges. Nevertheless, reciprocity does not systematically take place within the same type of support (material for material), but at a more global level (care for material for instance).

  2. (2b) As reciprocity is normatively expected and failing to reciprocate may lead to exclusion, it is systematic among personal configurations based on non-kin members, as these relationships are taken less for granted. In families, reciprocity is likely to unfold over larger periods of time and to be indirect (e.g., adult children getting financial help from their parents and later on financially helping their children).

  3. (2c) As configuration structures characterized by density of emotional support create a more normatively controlling environment, they are associated with more reciprocity, whereas configuration structures characterized by density of conflict are associated with less reciprocity.

5. Methods and measurements

5.1 Sample

This study used data from a Swiss national survey that was conducted in 2011, based on a representative sample of 803 individuals, born either between 1950 and 1955 (aged 56–61) or between 1970 and 1975 (aged 36–41), living in three major linguistic regions of Switzerland (German-, French-, and Italian-speaking regions). The survey had three main parts: personal configuration, retrospective life trajectories, and values. The cohort design was especially meaningful for the life trajectory part. The recruitment was made through the Swiss Federal Statistical Office, and a survey institute conducted face-to-face interviews using a computer-assisted personal interviewing method.

5.2 Respondents

There were 755 respondents who provided valid configuration data and reported at least one configuration member. Women composed 51% of the sample; 52% of respondents belonged to the 1950–55 cohort and 82% included Swiss citizens. Respondents with vocational education added up to 64%; furthermore, 20% had a tertiary education, 10% lower secondary education, and 7% upper secondary education. Among the respondents, 48% were employed full-time (80–100%), 22% were employed part-time (less than 80%), 15% were self-employed, 7% stayed at home, and 7% were in other situations.

5.3 Methodological approach

We first estimated the distribution of dyadic instrumental support exchanges between respondents and their configuration members (support relating to overall instrumental, financial, material, and care). Next, we considered whether these were reciprocal or unidirectional with the respondents being either mainly providers or recipients of help. For both aspects, we assessed the association with the whole configuration composition and structure by means of logistic regressions, including a set of sociodemographic covariates. All computations were made using the R statistical environment (R Development Core Team, 2020).

5.4 Instrumental support in dyadic exchanges

Dyadic instrumental support exchanges between respondents and their configuration members were investigated through three types of support: financial, material, and care. Financial support encompasses lending or giving money, inheritance or donation, offering partnership or passing on a business, giving a house or land. Material support encompasses giving clothes and food, helping to buy furniture and appliances, and with housing. Care support encompasses helping with the housework, in illness, taking care of alters, errands, helping in small repairs at home, and transport of alters. Both financial and material support requires more wealth than care support, but on a quite different scale. It was asked whether the respondents received or gave any of the three kinds of support to their configuration members. Partners and children aged under 25 were excluded because such exchanges are linked to sharing a common family household. Therefore, to measure such exchanges, it was necessary to eliminate cases that did not meet this criterion. We kept all respondents who had at least one configuration member outside the nuclear family; as for those entirely focused on the nuclear family, we kept those who had at least one child aged 25 or above (n = 672).

5.5 Configuration composition

Respondents were first asked to name “the individuals who, over the past year, have been very important to you, even if you have not gotten along well with them.” They could name up to 20 important alters (size ranged from 1 to 17 with a mean of 3.9 and a standard deviation of 2.15). For each of them, respondents were asked to define their relationship, choosing from an extensive list of 51 personal ties printed on a showcard.

A typology of personal configurations was built based on the different personal ties cited by the respondents to account for the diversity of arrangements. We used a standard two-step procedure combining principal components analysis and hierarchical clustering analysis [for details of the procedure see Widmer et al. (Reference Widmer, Aeby and Sapin2013), Aeby et al. (Reference Aeby, Gauthier and Widmer2019)]. The labels of the configurations were chosen to reflect the standpoint of the focal person, i.e., the respondents in relation to their configuration members. Labels represent the main orientation of the configurations, but do not exclude the presence of other ties. We ended up with seven personal configurations, by order of importance: Female friends and children based on female friendship and children, often without a partner (25%); Nuclear centered on the partner and minor and adult children (23%); Parents including mothers and fathers (13%); Siblings including sisters and brothers (12%); Male friends based on male friendship, often including a partner in contrast to the Female friends and children configuration (11%); Kinship including collaterals, siblings-in-law and other relatives (9%); Professional and non-kin including colleagues and other non-kin individuals (7%).

5.6 Configuration structure

To describe the structure of personal configuration, respondents were asked to report emotional support relationships, received and/or given, existing among all of their configuration members. The question started with, “Among the persons you have just mentioned, who could give you emotional support if needed?” and was followed by, “And what about the first (second, third,… nth) person you mentioned? Who could give him or her emotional support if needed (yourself included)?” Regarding conflict, respondents were asked to report who, among their configuration members, could anger or bother them and/or the other configuration members. Similarly, the question started with, “Among the persons you have just mentioned, who could anger you (annoy you)?” and was followed by, “And what about the first (second, third,… nth) person you mentioned? Who could anger him or her (annoy him or her) (yourself included)?” This approach goes beyond dyadic exchanges (between respondents and their configuration members) as it considers the whole configuration holistically. Following previous validation studies [Widmer (Reference Widmer2010)], configuration analysis measures were applied to investigate density and centrality in personal relationships [Wasserman and Faust (Reference Wasserman and Faust1994), Borgatti et al. (Reference Borgatti, Everett and Johnson2013)]. Density within the whole configuration corresponds to the number of actual connections reported by the respondent, divided by the maximum theoretical number of connections in a given configuration. The overall mean of support density was 0.62, much higher than the overall mean of conflict density (0.36). Betweenness centrality examines the extent to which an actor stands between all other actors within the same configuration [Freeman (Reference Freeman1978)]. It is computed as the number of shortest paths between configuration members that pass through this actor, corresponding here to the respondent. The overall mean of betweenness centrality was 0.26 for support and 0.11 for conflict.

5.7 Control variables

We included several control variables in the analyses, namely sex (women or men), birth cohort (1950–1955 or 1970–1975), education (lower secondary, upper secondary, vocational, and tertiary), and citizenship (being a Swiss national or not).

6. Results

In this section, we first estimate the prevalence of different types of instrumental support and then assess the direction of these exchanges, examining who provides or benefits from them. In both cases, we measure the influence played out by the personal configurations in the exchanges of support.

6.1 Financial, material, and care supports in personal configurations

Results show that circa 40% of the respondents (n = 271) were engaged in dyadic instrumental support exchanges with their configuration members (partners and children under 25 excluded). It implies that circa 60% of the respondents (n = 401) did not report such exchanges. Overall, a third of the respondents were involved in receiving (n = 213) and another third in giving (n = 226) at least one of the three kinds of support to their configuration members. Care support was the most common (26.2% reporting receiving care and 28% reporting giving care), whereas financial (10.3% reporting receiving care and 11.6% reporting giving care) and material support (11.5% reporting receiving care and 15% reporting giving care) were more exceptionally received and given, which confirms hypothesis 1a regarding the prevalence of care support.

We then looked at the absence or presence of instrumental support exchanges in the personal configurations across the full sample of 672 respondents. In all personal configurations, instrumental support exchanges took place, which means that no type of personal configuration was excluded from such exchanges. At the highest end, 58.2% of respondents with a Parent configuration were involved in such exchanges, whereas at the lowest end it was 22% of respondents with a Male friend configuration.

Evaluating the subsample of respondents who were involved in instrumental support exchanges (n = 271), we look closely at the distribution of the different types of support across personal configurations (see Table 1). Receiving varied between 19.2% in the Nuclear configurations and 54.1% in the Parent ones, while giving varied between 18% in Male friend configurations and 41.5% in Kinship ones. Those with the highest percentage of respondents involved in receiving financial and material support was the Parent configuration. Following the same downward direction from parents to children, the configurations with the highest percentage of respondents involved in giving financial support were the Nuclear ones. It shows the predominance of parents-adult children solidarity.

Table 1. Distribution of financial, material, and care supports among individuals who reported instrumental support exchanges by personal configurations (n = 271)

Note: Percentages do not sum up to 100, as individuals can be involved in different types of support exchanges.

In configurations composed of non-kin ties, for instance Professional and non-kin and Female friends and children configurations, instrumental support exchanges also took place. While financial support was less common, it did happen, indicating that non-kin ties may be mobilized if needed under certain circumstances. Care support was widespread in all types of personal configurations with the exception of the Male friend ones. Getting back to Hypothesis 1b, according to which personal configurations based on family members (especially parents and adult children) are more likely than non-kin to provide or be given financial or material support, the data confirm the prevalence of such exchanges in configurations including parent and adult children. Moreover, exchanges of financial and material support were well-spread in Female friends and children configurations as in Kinship configurations.

We then performed a series of logistic regressions on receiving (yes, no) and giving (yes, no) instrumental support indicators to assess their association with configuration composition and structure (see Table 2). Four control variables were used, namely sex, birth cohort, education, and citizenship. For configuration composition, we used the Nuclear configuration type as the reference as they represent a standard well-known kind. Results shown in Table 1 were corroborated. Indeed, in Parents configurations receiving and giving support overall was higher than in Nuclear configurations. Financial, material, and care supports were more frequently received by individuals in such configurations, showing downward as well as upward intergenerational solidarity. Kinship configurations were also positively associated with receiving support (overall, material, and care) and with giving support overall. Female friends and children configurations showed similar tendencies as Kinship ones, including for financial support. In contrast, most non-kin configurations (with the exception of Female friends and children) were more likely to be associated only with care exchanges.

Table 2. Configuration composition and structure with types of instrumental support exchanges, logistic regressions (odds ratios)

Sig.: p ≤ 0.1; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.

Considering the structure of personal configurations, density of emotional support was positively associated with giving care support, while betweenness of emotional support was negatively associated with receiving and giving financial support. Finally, density of conflict showed a negative association with receiving support overall and with care support in particular. Hypothesis 1c is confirmed, especially for care support. The negative association between centrality of emotional support and financial was not expected; we will return to that in Section 7.

Regarding control variables, the younger birth cohort was positively associated with receiving material support and negatively with giving financial and material support. This aspect underlines that intergenerational solidarity tends to be downward at younger ages.

To illustrate these results, we show the configurations of two individuals, a man and a woman, both born between 1970 and 1975, mentioning four important alters and having a Parent configuration. Individual 1 mentioned his partner, father, mother, and sister, while individual 2 mentioned her partner, friend, mother, and father. While in terms of composition, their configurations show similarities, they are quite different regarding their structure. The top parts a and b of Figure 1 illustrate the emotional support relationships, received and/or given, existing among all the configuration members, reported by these individuals (referred to as Ego). The configuration of individual 1 is characterized by a high density of support as all configuration members are connected (0.7) in contrast to the configuration of individual 2 (0.3). The bottom parts c and d of Figure 1 demonstrate the existing conflict relationships among all configuration members and themselves. Although individual 1 did not report any conflict (conflict density of 0), individual 2 reported them, especially between herself and her parents leading to a density of conflict of 0.2. When asked about instrumental exchanges, individual 1 reported receiving and giving care, financial, and material supports. In contrast, individual 2 only reported being able to receive material support, which shows the impact of the structure.

Figure 1. Personal configurations: (a) Structure of emotional support of individual 1 (male). (b) Structure of emotional support of individual 2 (female). (c) Structure of conflict support of individual 1 (male). (d). Structure of conflict of individual 2 (female).

6.2 Reciprocity of exchanges of instrumental support in personal configurations

Reciprocity implies that individuals who provide support receive it at some point and vice versa. Indeed, among the 271 respondents involved in dyadic instrumental support exchanges, 61.8% were involved in reciprocal exchanges, while 21.7% were only support providers and 16.5% only support recipients (see Table 3). This underlines the overall prominence of reciprocity in such relationships. However, when investigating within each type of support, other patterns emerged. Reciprocity was low for financial support (13.1%), medium for material (32.6%), and high for care (65.5%). Giving or receiving financial support implies that the recipient is in a durable situation of need that makes it difficult or impossible to reciprocate. Consequently, some objects of reciprocity (e.g., money) are logically less required, but a global equilibrium is often achieved engaging other kinds of support, as the general pattern of reciprocity indicates. Those findings substantiate Hypothesis 2a about the prevalence of reciprocity over unidirectional exchanges, and the fact that reciprocity is higher at a global level than within each type of support considered separately.

Table 3. Distribution of reciprocity in instrumental support exchanges: overall, financial, material and care supports

In a second step, we performed a set of logistic regressions on reciprocity indicators to assess their association with configurations' composition and structure (see Table 4). Regarding the composition of personal configurations, Parents configurations were not statistically associated with reciprocity overall, but interestingly they were significantly less likely to be associated with providers of support only. Kinship and Professional configurations were three to four times more likely to be associated with reciprocity overall compared to Nuclear configurations. The former was, however, less likely to be associated with support-only providers, as it was the case for Female friends and children configurations. This confirms Hypothesis 2b, according to which reciprocity is more likely among personal configurations based on non-kin members.

Table 4. Configuration composition and structure with reciprocity, logistic regressions (odds-ratios)

Sig.: † p ≤ 0.1; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.

Finally, configuration structures characterized by density of emotional support were positively associated with reciprocity. Conflict did not account for the directions of exchanges, indirectly underlying that reciprocity is expected independently of the quality of the relationships. Hypothesis 2c is confirmed regarding the positive impact of the density of emotional support, but not concerning the negative association of density of conflict. For control variables, the younger birth cohort was negatively associated with being a provider-only. This aspect confirms again that intergenerational solidarity tends to be downward at younger ages.

7. Discussion

We addressed the issue of instrumental support exchanges embedded in personal configurations. Overall, a third of the respondents reported being involved in instrumental support exchanges with their significant others outside the nuclear family, which means that two-thirds were not. This seemingly low percentage highlights that in Switzerland, as in most Western contemporary societies, instrumental support tends to be limited to situations of manifest need [Swartz (Reference Swartz2009)]. This percentage has to be put in perspective with the Swiss context characterized by long-term socioeconomic stability and a welfare state providing basic social coverage [Obinger (Reference Obinger1998), Wall et al. (Reference Wall, Widmer, Gauthier, Česnuitytė and Gouveia2018)]. However, this percentage is not a negligible one and it does require our full attention.

As instrumental support exchanges between two individuals are embedded in wider webs of interdependencies, we considered the characteristics of personal configurations that may enhance or hinder them. The composition of personal configurations undoubtedly mattered. Indeed, instrumental support in personal configurations including parents and adult children was common. It confirms that support, especially financial, does not stop when children become adults and leave the parental nest [Bonvalet and Ogg (Reference Bonvalet and Ogg2007)]. Moreover, configurations giving room to extended kinship were a source of support. Irrespective of the exact type of tie, being related guarantees a space of safe exchanges governed by family solidarity rules. This highlights the fact that family relationships have a latent function embodying a “family national guard” [Hagestad (Reference Hagestad and Bengtson1996)] or a “support bank” [Antonucci and Jackson (Reference Antonucci, Jackson and Warnes1989)].

Latency is important outside the realm of the family as it has been underlined with the idea of fossil friendships that one can reactivate when needed [Pahl (Reference Pahl2000)]. Personal configurations based on non-kin and especially those including female friendships proved to be significantly supportive. A process of suffusion can be witnessed [Pahl and Spencer (Reference Pahl and Spencer2004)], if financial support is put aside. The absence of partner and kin ties reinforces the importance of friendship. Friendship is prominent in contexts of singlehood, for instance [Bellotti (Reference Bellotti2008)]. Overall, all types of personal configurations could be a source of instrumental support, in particular for care support. This is an important finding as it shows that there is no type of personal configuration that is associated with an exclusion from support mechanisms. Nevertheless, as pointed above, the type and amount of support vary significantly depending on composition. One has to keep in mind that composition of personal configurations reflects individuals' overall embeddedness in society and may vary following important life-course stages, transitions, and events [Antonucci et al. (Reference Antonucci, Fiori, Birditt, Jackey, Lerner and Overton2010), Levy and Widmer (Reference Levy and Widmer2013), Aeby et al. (Reference Aeby, Gauthier and Widmer2019)]. It implies that in case of crisis such as an intimate relationship breakdown, some close friends can reveal themselves disappointingly less supportive than expected [Aeby and Heath (Reference Aeby and Heath2020)] or, contrastingly, that marital instability of adult children can positively reinforce support to parents and feelings of filial responsibility [Swartz (Reference Swartz2009)]. It means that over the life course, individuals may have to re-evaluate their relationships and may drop conflictual ones and replace them with new supportive ones.

Reciprocity is a key feature of those instrumental support exchanges. It is an organizing principle of exchanges in society in general, as well as among close individuals [Molm et al. (Reference Molm, Collett and Schaefer2007), Girardin (Reference Girardin2017)]. However, it did not systematically happen within the same type of support, which emphasizes the importance of global reciprocity in personal relationships, rather than direct reciprocity. Financial support often cannot be returned, but can be reciprocated by care services. While the percentage of reciprocity is high, some respondents found themselves in the position of a provider or recipient of support. This could be linked to differences in resources, as well as to the fact that in situations of a chain-generalized reciprocity where recipients and benefactors know each other [Molm et al. (Reference Molm, Collett and Schaefer2007)], some (younger) individuals have only experienced the interplay of support in one position of the chain yet, but they may experience it in a different way in the future. Regarding such exchanges, it is also interesting to highlight that we did not find any difference based on economic means, proxied by the level of education. Offer (Reference Offer2012) explained that individuals may withdraw from exchanges when they cannot reciprocate, rather than becoming exclusively a recipient. Being a recipient only is accepted for children toward parents, but less so for other types of ties. This intergenerational solidarity is accountable for some of the social inequalities being maintained over time [Chauvel (Reference Chauvel, Culpepper, Hall and Palier2006), Swartz (Reference Swartz2009)].

Finally, the structure of personal configurations extended the understanding of instrumental support exchanges. Emotionally dense structures that provide bonding social capital [Coleman (Reference Coleman1988), Widmer (Reference Widmer2010)] proved to be associated with giving care and reciprocity overall. This protective but also controlling environment creates a collective normative pressure to ensure that individuals comply with their obligation of giving. This confirms the weight of support norms, existing in most Western societies with their specificities according to types of welfare regime [Widmer and Ganjour (Reference Widmer and Ganjour2016)]. Contrastingly, in emotionally centralized structures producing bridging social capital where respondents represent a bridge between distinct configuration members [Burt (Reference Burt, Guillén, Collins, England and Meyer2002), Widmer (Reference Widmer2010)], giving financial support was less frequent. This may be due to an impossibility to be simultaneously at the center of emotional and financial flows. In personal configurations producing bonding social capital, individuals were less likely to be providers only. This shows that individuals in emotionally dense supportive environment are less likely to feel drained and report an imbalance toward giving only. Personal configurations characterized by density of conflict were less associated with receiving, but this did not influence reciprocity exchanges in general. It underlines that reciprocity does not uniquely respond to supportive relationships, as most close relationships are characterized by ambivalence [Lüscher and Pillemer (Reference Lüscher and Pillemer1998), Aeby et al. (Reference Aeby, Gauthier and Widmer2020)]. Supportive and conflictual relationships tend to co-exist in personal configurations, while solely conflictual ones tend to be dropped [Uchino et al. (Reference Uchino, Holt-Lunstad, Smith and Bloor2004)].

This study has some limitations. First, the analyses for reciprocity were performed on a small number of individuals who were engaged in instrumental support exchanges. Second, the personal configurations were collected from single informants who gave their perception of the relationships involving themselves and their configuration members. Finally, regarding the differences between the type of supports and the type of ties, as we did not test each type of ties separately, but did it by configuration, we cannot provide a answer to the roles specifically played by friends. Despite these limitations, this study sheds new light on instrumental support exchanges by integrating them in a configurational approach that includes non-kin ties and by considering simultaneously the composition and structure of personal configurations.

Acknowledgements

This publication benefited from the support Swiss National Science Foundation (grant number: 100017_130343/1), and of the Swiss National Centre of Competence in Research LIVES—Overcoming vulnerability: Life course perspectives (NCCR LIVES), which is financed by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant number: 51NF40-160590). The authors are grateful to the Swiss National Science Foundation for its financial assistance.

References

Aeby, Gaëlle, Gauthier, Jacques-Antoine and Widmer, Eric D. (2019) Beyond the nuclear family: personal networks in light of work-family trajectories. Advances in Life Course Research 39, 5160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aeby, Gaëlle, Gauthier, Jacques-Antoine and Widmer, Eric D. (2020) Patterns of support and conflict relationships in personal networks and perceived stress. Current Sociology. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011392120948926.Google Scholar
Aeby, Gaëlle and Heath, Sue (2020) Post break-up housing pathways of young adults in England in light of family and friendship-based support. Journal of Youth Studies 23(10), 13811395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allan, Graham (2008) Flexibility, friendship, and family. Personal Relationships 15(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antonucci, Toni C., Fiori, Katherine L., Birditt, Kira and Jackey, Lisa M. H. (2010) Convoys of social relations: integrating life-span and life-course perspectives. In Lerner, Richard M. and Overton, Willis F (ed.), The Handbook of Life-Span Development, pp. 434473. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Antonucci, Toni C. and Jackson, J. S. (1989) Successful aging and life course reciprocity. In Warnes, Anthony M. (ed.) Human Ageing and Later Life: Multidisciplinary Perspectives, pp. 8395. London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
Bellotti, Elisa (2008) What are friends for? Elective communities of single people. Social Networks 30(4), 318329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bengtson, Vern L (2001) Beyond the nuclear family: the increasing importance of multigenerational bonds (The burgess award lecture). Journal of Marriage and Family 63(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonvalet, Catherine, and Ogg, Jim, (ed.) (2007) Measuring Family Support in Europe. London: Southern Universities Press.Google Scholar
Borgatti, Stephen P., Everett, Martin G. and Johnson, Jefferey C. (2013) Analyzing Social Networks. Los Angeles: SAGE.Google Scholar
Burt, Ronald S. (2002) The social capital of structural holes. In Guillén, Mauro F, Collins, Randall, England, Paula, and Meyer, M. (ed.), The New Economic Sociology: Developments in an Emerging Field, pp. 148190. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Chauvel, Louis (2006) Social generations, life chances and welfare regime sustainability. In Culpepper, Pepper D., Hall, Peter A., and Palier, Bruno (ed.), Changing France: The Politics That Markets Make, pp. 150175. French Politics, Society and Culture Series. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK.Google Scholar
Coenen-Huther, Josette, Kellerhals, Jean, von Allmen, Malik, Hagmann, Hermann-Michel, Jeannerat, Fabienne and Widmer, Eric D. (1994) Les Réseaux de Solidarité Dans la Famille. Lausanne: Réalités sociales.Google Scholar
Cohen, Sheldon (2004) Social relationships and health. The American Psychologist 59(8), 676684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coleman, James S (1988) Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology 94(janvier), 95120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conkova, Nina, Fokkema, Tineke and Dykstra, Pearl A. (2018) Non-kin ties as a source of support in Europe: understanding the role of cultural context. European Societies 20(1), 131156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, Karen S. and Emerson, Richard M. (1978) Power, equity and commitment in exchange networks. American Sociological Review 43(5), 721739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornwell, Benjamin (2011) Independence through social networks: bridging potential among older women and men. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 66B(6), 782794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elias, Norbert (1978) What Is Sociology? New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Elias, Norbert (1983) The Court Society. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fingerman, Karen L., Hay, Elizabeth L. and Birditt, Kira S. (2004) The best of ties, the worst of ties: close, problematic, and ambivalent social relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family 66(3), 792808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, Linton C (1978) Centrality in social networks conceptual clarification. Social Networks 1(3), 215239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girardin, Myriam (2017) Les configurations familiales aux dernières étapes de la vie. PhD Thesis, Genève: Université de Genève, Faculté des sciences de la société.Google Scholar
Hagestad, G. O. (1996) On-Time, off-time, out of time? Reflections on continuity and discontinuity from an illness process. In Bengtson, Vern L. (ed.), Adulthood and Aging: Research on Continuities and Discontinuities, pp. 204222. New York: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Heath, Sue, Davies, Katherine, Edwards, Gemma and Scicluna, Rachael (2017) Shared Housing, Shared Lives : Everyday Experiences Across the Lifecourse. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kadushin, Charles (2002) The motivational foundation of social networks. Social Networks 24(1), 7791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohli, Martin (2007) The institutionalization of the life course: looking back to look ahead. Research in Human Development 4(3 4), 253271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, René, and Widmer, Eric D. (ed.) (2013) Gendered Life Courses Between Standardization and Individualization: A European Approach Applied to Switzerland. Wien: LIT Verlag.Google Scholar
Lubbers, Miranda J., Molina, José Luis, Lerner, Jürgen, Brandes, Ulrik, Ávila, Javier and McCarty, Christopher (2010) Longitudinal analysis of personal networks. The case of Argentinean migrants in Spain. Social Networks 32(1), 91104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüscher, Kurt and Pillemer, Karl (1998) Intergenerational ambivalence: a new approach to the study of parent-child relations in later life. Journal of Marriage and Family 60(2), 413425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauss, Marcel (2007) Essai sur le don: forme et raison de l’échange dans les sociétés archaïques. Quadrige. Grands textes. Paris: Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
McDonald, Steve and Mair, Christine A (2010) Social capital across the life course: age and gendered patterns of network resources. Sociological Forum 25(2), 335359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, Miller, Smith-Lovin, Lynn and Brashears, Matthew E. (2006) Social isolation in America: changes in core discussion networks over two decades. American Sociological Review 71(3), 353375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, Miller, Smith-Lovin, Lynn and Cook, James M. (2001) Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology 27(1), 415444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molm, Linda D., Collett, Jessica L. and Schaefer, David R. (2007) Building solidarity through generalized exchange: a theory of reciprocity. American Journal of Sociology 113(1), 205242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolaisen, Magnhild and Thorsen, Kirsten (2017) What are friends for? Friendships and loneliness over the lifespan—from 18 to 79 years. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development 84(2), 126158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obinger, Herbert (1998) Federalism, direct democracy, and welfare state development in Switzerland. Journal of Public Policy 18(03), 241263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Offer, Shira (2012) The burden of reciprocity: processes of exclusion and withdrawal from personal networks among low-income families. Current Sociology 60(6), 788805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pahl, R. E. (2000) On Friendship. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Pahl, Ray and Spencer, Liz (2004) Personal communities: not simply families of ‘fate’ or ‘choice’. Current Sociology 52(2), 199221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, Talcott (1949) The social structure of the family. In Nanda Anshen, Ruth (ed.) The Family: Its Function and Destiny, pp. 173201. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team (2020) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Richards, Lindsay (2016) For whom money matters less: social connectedness as a resilience resource in the UK. Social Indicators Research 125(2), 509535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riley, Matilda White and Riley, J. W (1993) Connections: kin and cohort. In Achenbaum, W. Andrew and Bengtson, Vern L. (ed.), The Changing Contract Across Generations, pp. 169189. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Ryan, Louise, Sales, Rosemary, Tilki, Mary and Siara, Bernadetta (2008) Social networks, social support and social capital: the experiences of recent polish migrants in London. Sociology 42(4), 672690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapin, Marlène, Widmer, Eric D. and Iglesias, Katia (2016) From support to overload: patterns of positive and negative family relationships of adults with mental illness over time. Social Networks 47, 5972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smart, Carol (2010) Personal Life: New Directions in Sociological Thinking. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Spini, Dario, Bernardi, Laura and Oris, Michel (2017) Toward a life course framework for studying vulnerability. Research in Human Development 14(1), 525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suanet, Bianca and Antonucci, Toni C. (2017) Cohort differences in received social support in later life: the role of network type. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 72(4), 706715.Google ScholarPubMed
Swartz, Teresa Toguchi (2009) Intergenerational family relations in adulthood: patterns, variations, and implications in the contemporary United States. Annual Review of Sociology 35, 191212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uchino, Bert N., Holt-Lunstad, Julianne, Smith, Timothy W. and Bloor, Lindsey (2004) Heterogeneity in social networks: a comparison of different models linking relationships to psychological outcomes. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 23(2), 123139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Gaag, Martin and Snijders, Tom A.B. (2005) The resource generator: social capital quantification with concrete items. Social Networks 27(1), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wall, Karin and Gouveia, Rita (2014) Changing meanings of family in personal relationships. Current Sociology 62(3), 352373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wall, Karin, Widmer, Eric D., Gauthier, Jacques-Antoine, Česnuitytė, Vida and Gouveia, Rita (2018) Families and Personal Networks An International Comparative Perspective. London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasserman, Stanley and Faust, Katherine (1994) Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications. Structural Analysis in the Social Sciences 8. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weeks, Jeffrey, Heaphy, Brian and Donovan, Catherine (2001) Same Sex Intimacies: Families of Choice and Other Life Experiments. London, New York: Routlede.Google Scholar
Wellman, Barry and Wortley, Scot (1990) Different strokes from different folks: community ties and social support. American Journal of Sociology 96(3), 558588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widmer, Eric D. (2010) Family Configurations: A Structural Approach to Family Diversity. Farnham: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Widmer, Eric D., Aeby, Gaëlle and Sapin, Marlène (2013) Collecting family network data. International Review of Sociology 23(1), 2746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widmer, Eric D., de Bel, V., Olga Ganjour, Myriam Girardin and Zufferey, Marie-Eve (2020) Dynamiques familiales et COVID-19: réactions à la période de confinement. In Gamba, Fiorenza, Nardone, Marco, Cattacin, Sandro, and Ricciardi, Toni (ed.), Covid-19 : Le regard des sciences sociales, pp. 159177. Zürich: Editions Seismo.Google Scholar
Widmer, Eric D. and Ganjour, Olga (2016) Patterns of family salience and welfare state regimes: sociability practices and support norms in a comparative perspective. European Societies 18(3), 201220.Google Scholar
Widmer, Eric D, Girardin, Myriam and Ludwig, Catherine (2018) Conflict structures in family networks of older adults and their relationship with health-related quality of life. Journal of Family Issues 39(6), 15731597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widmer, Eric D. and Lüscher, Kurt (2011) Les relations intergénérationelles au prisme de l'ambivalence et des configurations familiales. Recherches Familiales 8, 4960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyss, Malika and Nedelcu, Mihaela (2020) Grandparents on the move: a multilevel framework analysis to understand diversity in zero-generation care arrangements in Switzerland. Global Networks 20(2), 343361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Distribution of financial, material, and care supports among individuals who reported instrumental support exchanges by personal configurations (n = 271)

Figure 1

Table 2. Configuration composition and structure with types of instrumental support exchanges, logistic regressions (odds ratios)

Figure 2

Figure 1. Personal configurations: (a) Structure of emotional support of individual 1 (male). (b) Structure of emotional support of individual 2 (female). (c) Structure of conflict support of individual 1 (male). (d). Structure of conflict of individual 2 (female).

Figure 3

Table 3. Distribution of reciprocity in instrumental support exchanges: overall, financial, material and care supports

Figure 4

Table 4. Configuration composition and structure with reciprocity, logistic regressions (odds-ratios)