Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-g78kv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-28T07:22:21.619Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Tagging after red herrings: evidence against the processing capacity explanation in child language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Peyton Todd
Affiliation:
The University of Chicago

Abstract

A case is reported of failure to supply negation in tag questions for a period of nearly two years. The duration of this error shows that it could not have been the result of limited processing capacity, and this provides reason to doubt this explanation for the similar case of children's failure to invert subject and auxiliary verb in wh-questions. It is argued that other cases which have been explained in terms of limited processing capacity are equally compatible with an explanation in terms of context-specific knowledge, which holds that a rule is simply not known for contexts other than those where it is expressed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Antinucci, F. & Parisi, D. (1973). Early language acquisition: a model and some data. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I.. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Antinucci, F. (1975). Early semantic development in child language. In Lenneberg, E. H. & Lenneberg, E.. (eds), Foundations of language development: a multidisciplinary approach. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U. (1966). Linguistic mechanisms underlying child speech. In Zale, E. M.. (ed.), Proceedings of the conference on language and language behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U. (1967). The acquisition of the system of negation in children's speech. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U. (1971). Simplification in children's language. In Huxley, R. & Ingram, E.. (eds), Language acquisition: models and methods. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single-word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1978). Semantic and syntactic development. In Schiefelbusch, R.. (ed.), The bases of language intervention. College Park, Md.: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1974). Length constraints, reduction rules, and holophrastic processes in children's word combinations. JVLVB 13. 448–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children's first word combinations. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 41 (1), no. 164.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge. Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., Cazden, C. & Bellugi, U. (1968). The child's grammar from I to III. In Hill, J. P.. (ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. R.. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cazden, C. (1968). The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. ChDev 38. 433–8.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. (1970). Language and consciousness. Lg 50. 111–33.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1974). Performing without competence. JChLang 1. 110.Google Scholar
Cox, D. R. (1970). The analysis of binary data. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S. (1976). Language development: structure and function. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. & de Villiers, P. (1973). A cross-sectional study on the development of grammatical morphemes in child speech. JPsycholingRes 2. 267–78.Google ScholarPubMed
Dore, J., Franklin, M., Miller, R. & Ramer, A. (1976). Transitional phenomena in early language acquisition. JChLang 3. 1328.Google Scholar
Ervin, S. (1964). Imitation and structural change in children's language. In Lenneberg, E. H.. (ed.), New directions in the study of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1971). Transitivity in child language. Lg 47. 888909.Google Scholar
Knapp, D. (1978). Automatization and language acquisition. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Child Language Development.Google Scholar
Labov, W. & Labov, T. (1976). Learning the syntax of questions. Paper given to the conference on the psychology of language, Stirling.Google Scholar
Maxwell, A. D. (1961). Analyzing qualitative data. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1966). Developmental psycholinguistics. In Smith, F. & Miller, G. A.. (eds), The genesis of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1970). The acquisition of language. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Nelder, J. A. & Wedderburn, R. W. M. (1972). Generalized linear models. JRoyStatSoc Series A 135. 370–84.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Humanities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1941). Le développement des quantités chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1966). The acquisition of Russian as a native language. In Smith, F. & Miller, G. A.. (eds), The genesis of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1979). Psycholinguistics. Glenview, Ill.: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. & Welsh, C. A. (1973). Elicited imitation as a research tool in developmental psycholinguistics. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I.. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar