Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-68ccn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-09T20:13:58.690Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A note on children's lexical representations*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Marilyn May Vihman
Affiliation:
Stanford University

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Notes and Discussion
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Barton, D. (1978). The role of perception in the acquisition of phonology. Bloomington: Indiana Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Review article: The acquisition of phonology by N. V. Smith. Lg 52. 489–98.Google Scholar
Dodd, B. (1975). Children's understanding of their own phonological forms. QJExpPsych 27. 165–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Fant, G. (1973). Speech sounds and features. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1978). Fricatives in child language acquisition. In Honsa, V. & Hardman-de-Bautista, M. J. (eds), Papers on linguistics and child language. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Goldstein, L. (1977). Feature salience and bias. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 39.Google Scholar
Graham, L. W. & House, A. S. (1971). Phonological oppositions in children: a perceptual study. JAcS 49. 559–66.Google ScholarPubMed
Harris, K. S. (1958). Cues for the discrimination of American English fricatives in spoken syllables. L&S 1. 17.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. F. (1947). Speech development of a bilingual child. Vol. 2, Sound-learning in the first two years. Evanston: Northwestern University.Google Scholar
Macken, M. A. (1980). The child's lexical representation: the ‘puzzle-puddle-pickle’ evidence. JLing 16. 117.Google Scholar
Menn, L. (1979). Towards a psychology of phonology: child phonology as a first step. Paper presented at the Conference on Applications of Linguistic Theory in the Human Sciences.Michigan State University.Google Scholar
Miller, G. & Nicely, P. (1955). An analysis of perceptual confusions among English consonants. JAcS 27. 338–52.Google Scholar
Read, C. (1975). Children's categorization of speech sounds in English. National Council of Teachers of English Report 7.Google Scholar
Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R. J. & Levelt, W. J. M. (1978). The child's conception of language. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: a case study. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Smith, N. V. (1978). Lexical representation and the acquisition of phonology. Forum lecture delivered at the Linguistic Institute of the LSA at Urbana, Illinois, July.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1976). From pre-speech to speech: on early phonology. PRCLD 12. 230–43.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1978). Consonant harmony: its scope and function in child language. In Greenberg, J. H.. (ed.), Universals of human language. Vol. 2. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1980). Formulas in first and second language acquisition. PRCLD 18. 7592. To appear in Obler, L. & Menn, L.. (eds), Exceptional language and linguistics (New York: Academic Press)Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1981). Phonology and the development of the lexicon: evidence from children's errors. JChLang 8. 239–64.Google ScholarPubMed