Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T21:01:52.667Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal interactive beliefs and style as predictors of language development in preterm and full term children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 June 2020

Sharifeh YOUNESIAN*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health, School of Psychology and Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Australia Speech Therapy Department, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
Areana EIVERS
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health, School of Psychology and Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Australia
Ameneh SHAHAEIAN
Affiliation:
Institute for Learning Sciences and Teacher Education, Australian Catholic University
Karen SULLIVAN
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health, School of Psychology and Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Australia
Linda GILMORE
Affiliation:
School of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Sharifeh Younesian Faculty of Health School of Psychology and Counselling Queensland University of Technology Kelvin Grove Campus Victoria Park Road Kelvin Grove QLD 4059BrisbaneAustralia E-mail: sharifeh.younesian@connect.qut.edu.au

Abstract

Previous research has shown that the quality of mother-child interactions between pre-term children and their mothers tends to be poorer than that of full-term children and their mothers (Forcada-Guex, Pierrehumbert, Borghini, Moessinger & Muller-Nix, 2006). Mothers of pre-term children are less responsive and more intrusive in interactions with their children than mothers of full-term children (Forcada-Guex et al., 2006; Ionio, Lista, Mascheroni, Olivari, Confalonieri, Mastrangelo, Brazzoduro, Balestriero, Banfi, Bonanomi, Bova, Castoldi, Colombo, Introvini & Scelsa, 2017; Laing, McMahon, Ungerer, Taylor, Badawi & Spence, 2010). The current research explored differences between mothers of pre-term and full-term children in terms of interactive beliefs and style, and the potential for language development to be differentially predicted by maternal interactive beliefs and styles in pre-term versus full-term children. Independent t-tests were conducted to compare pre-term and full-term groups in relation to the measures of maternal interactive beliefs and styles. A series of multiple regression analyses were then performed separately for each group to examine the shared and unique contributions of maternal interactive beliefs and styles on full-term versus pre-term children's language development. The results showed that mothers of pre-term children were more intrusive-directive than mothers of full-term children; in contrast, mothers of full-term children were more responsive and supportive-directive in interactions with their children. Moreover, predictors of language development were different in full-term versus pre-term children; in full-term children, maternal supportive beliefs and responsiveness were significant predictors of language development evaluated by both the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development and the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory; in the pre-term group, maternal supportive and directive beliefs, as well as supportive and intrusive directiveness, were significant predictors, with the latter being negatively associated with language development indicators. This research can shed light on how to prevent language delay in children and improve mother-child interactions that contribute to language development, which may in turn improve language development in vulnerable children, children born pre-term in particular.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Aoki, M. F., & Rauh, V. A. (1993). Nine-year outcome of the Vermont intervention program for low birth weight infants. Pediatrics, 91(1 I), 4555.Google ScholarPubMed
Adams-Chapman, I., Bann, C., Carter, S. L., Stoll, B. J., & Network, N. N. R. (2015). Language outcomes among ELBW infants in early childhood. Early Human Development, 91(6), 373379. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.03.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alizadeh, H., & Andries, C. (2002). Interaction of parenting styles and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Iranian parents. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 24(3), 3752. doi:10.1300/J019v24n03_03CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apgar, V. (2015). A Proposal for a New Method of Evaluation of the Newborn Infant. Anesthesia & Analgesia, 120(5), 10561059. doi:10.1213/ANE.0b013e31829bdc5cCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arriaga, R. I., Fenson, L., Cronan, T., & Pethick, S. J. (1998). Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(2), 209223. doi:10.1017/S0142716400010043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M., Harris, M., & Chasin, J. (1991). Early lexical development and maternal speech: A comparison of children's initial and subsequent uses of words. Journal of child language, 18(1), 2140. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition: Pearson.Google Scholar
Behzadi, K. (1994). Interpersonal conflict and emotions in an Iranian cultural practice: Qahr andashti. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 18(3), 321359. doi:10.1007/bf01379230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Marinus, H. v. I. (2007). For Better and for Worse: Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 300304. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhutta, A. T., Cleves, M. A., Casey, P. H., Cradock, M. M., & Anand, K. J. S. (2002). Cognitive and Behavioral Outcomes of School-Aged Children Who Were Born Pre-term: A Meta-analysis. JAMA, 288(6), 728737. doi:10.1001/jama.288.6.728CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24(72,Pt3), 267304. doi:10.1177/0142723704045681CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., & Venuti, P. (2001). Parenting beliefs and behaviours in northern and southern groups of Italian mothers of young infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 15(4), 663675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal Responsiveness to Young Children at Three Ages: Longitudinal Analysis of a Multidimensional, Modular, and Specific Parenting Construct. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 867874. doi:10.1037/0012–1649.44.3.867CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouchard, C., Trudeau, N., Sutton, A., Boudreault, M.-C., & Deneault, J. (2009). Gender differences in language development in French Canadian children between 8 and 30 months of age. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(4), 685707. doi:10.1017/S0142716409990075CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, N. C., Doyle, L. W., Bear, M. J., & Inder, T. E. (2006). Alterations in Neurobehavior at Term Reflect Differing Perinatal Exposures in Very Pre-term Infants. Pediatrics, 118(6), 24612471. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-0880CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Language and Communication, 1(2), 155178. doi:10.1016/0271-5309(81)90010-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Development, 72(6), 18321843. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clegg, J., Hollis, C., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2005). Developmental language disorders – a follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(2), 128149. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00342.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crnic, K. A., Ragozin, A. S., Greenberg, M. T., Robinson, N. M., & Basham, R. B. (1983). Social interaction and developmental competence of pre-term and full-term infants during the first year of life. Child Development, 54(5), 1199. doi:10.2307/1129675CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Paradise, J. L., Feldman, H. M., Janosky, J. E., Pitcairn, D. N., & Kurs-Lasky, M. (1999). Maternal Education and Measures of Early Speech and Language.(Statistical Data Included). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(6), 1432. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4206.1432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donahue, M. L., Pearl, R., & Herzog, A. (1997). Mothers' referential communication with preschoolers: Effects of children's syntax and mothers' beliefs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 133147. doi:10.1016/S0193-3973(97)90019-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pereira, M. P., Wehberg, S., & Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 326343. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falkus, G., Tilley, C., Thomas, C., Hockey, H., Kennedy, A., Arnold, T., & Pring, T. (2016). Assessing the effectiveness of parent-child interaction therapy with language delayed children: A clinical investigation. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 32(1), 717. doi:10.1177/0265659015574918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feeley, N., Gottlieb, L., & Zelkowitz, P. (2007). Mothers and Fathers of Very Low–Birthweight Infants: Similarities and Differences in the First Year After Birth. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, & Neonatal Nursing, 36(6), 558567. doi:10.1111/j.1552-6909.2007.00186.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R. (2007). Maternal versus child risk and the development of parent-child and family relationships in five high-risk populations. Development and Psychopathology, 19(2), 293312. doi:10.1017/S0954579407070150CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(1), 95116. doi:10.1017/S0142716400001053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S. J., & Reilly, J. S. (1991). Technical manual for the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. San Diego State University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234248. doi:10.1111/desc.12019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, V., & Masur, E. F. (2007). Characteristics of maternal verbal style: Responsiveness and directiveness in two natural contexts. Journal of Child Language, 34(3), 519543. doi:10.1017/S030500090700801XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Borghini, A., Moessinger, A., & Muller-Nix, C. (2006). Early Dyadic Patterns of Mother-Infant Interactions and Outcomes of Prematurity at 18 Months. Pediatrics, 118(1), e107-e114. doi:10.1542/peds.2005–1145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster-Cohen, S. H., Friesen, M. D., Champion, P. R., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very pre-term. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 31(8), 658667. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7eCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, G., Plunkett, S. W., & Otten, M. P. (2010). Perceived parenting, self-esteem, and general self-efficacy of Iranian American adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19(6), 738746. doi:10.1007/s10826-010-9363-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H., & Forrester, M. (2009). Analysing interactions in childhood: Insights from conversation analysis (Vol. 1. Aufl.; 1). US: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ghorbani, N., Bing, M. N., Watson, P., Davison, H., & LeBreton, D. L. (2003). Individualist and collectivist values: Evidence of compatibility in Iran and the United States. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(2), 431447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grantham-McGregor, S., Cheung, Y. B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter, L., & Strupp, B. (2007). Child development in developing countries 1: Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. The Lancet, 369(9555), 6070. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60032-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, M. T., Carmichael-Olson, H., & Crnic, K. (1992). The development and social competence of a pre-term sample at age 4: Prediction and transactional outcomes. In Friedman, S & Sigman's, M (Eds), The psychological development of low-birthweight children, p. 125155.Google Scholar
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1996). Parents' cultural belief systems: their origins, expressions, and consequences. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Heilmann, J., Weismer, S. E., Evans, J., & Hollar, C. (2005). Utility of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory in Identifying Language Abilities of Late-Talking and Typically Developing Toddlers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(1), 4051. doi:10.1044/1058–0360(2005/006)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2003). The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development Via Maternal Speech. Child Development, 74(5), 13681378. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73, 418433. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00415CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imgrund, C. M. (2013). Directive language input to children born pre-term and full term. (Dissertation/Thesis), ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.Google Scholar
Ionio, C., Colombo, C., Brazzoduro, V., Mascheroni, E., Confalonieri, E., Castoldi, F., & Lista, G. (2016). Mothers and fathers in nicu: The impact of pre-term birth on parental distress. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 12(4), 604621. doi:10.5964/ejop.v12i4.1093CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ionio, C., & Di Blasio, P. (2014). Post-traumatic stress symptoms after childbirth and early mother-child interactions: an exploratory study. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 32(2), 163181. doi:10.1080/02646838.2013.841880CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ionio, C., Lista, G., Mascheroni, E., Olivari, M. G., Confalonieri, E., Mastrangelo, M., Brazzoduro, V., Balestriero, M., Banfi, A., Bonanomi, A., Bova, S., Castoldi, F., Colombo, C., Introvini, P., & Scelsa, B. (2017). Premature birth: complexities and difficulties in building the mother-child relationship. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 35(5), 509523. doi:10.1080/02646838.2017.1383977CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, J. R., & Wong, M. Y. A. (2002). Cultural differences in beliefs and practices concerning talk to children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5), 916926. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2002/074)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazemi, Y., Stringer, H., & Klee, T. (2015). Study of child language development and disorders in Iran: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of research in medical sciences : the official journal of Isfahan University of Medical Sciences, 20(1), 6677.Google ScholarPubMed
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child Temperament Moderates Effects of Parent–Child Mutuality on Self-Regulation: A Relationship-Based Path for Emotionally Negative Infants. Child Development, 83(4), 12751289. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01778.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. (1993). Sex differences in the brain Mind and brain: Readings from Scientific American magazine, (pp. 7989). New York, NY, US: W H Freeman/Times Books/ Henry Holt & Co.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., Barry, R. A., & Philibert, R. A. (2011). Children's genotypes interact with maternal responsive care in predicting children's competence: Diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility? Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 605616. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korja, R., Ahlqvist-Björkroth, S., Savonlahti, E., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., Piha, J., & Lehtonen, L. (2010). Relations between maternal attachment representations and the quality of mother–infant interaction in pre-term and full-term infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(3), 330336. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laing, S., McMahon, C., Ungerer, J., Taylor, A., Badawi, N., & Spence, K. (2010). Mother–child interaction and child developmental capacities in toddlers with major birth defects requiring newborn surgery. Early Human Development, 86(12), 793800. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.08.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L., & Swank, P. R. (1997). Predicting cognitive-language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 33, 10401053. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.1040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology, 42, 627642. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children's development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Developmental Psychology, 37, 387403. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.387CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Last, B. F., Schuengel, C., Kok, J. H., Houtzager, B. A., Wassenaer, v. A. G., & Potharst, E. S. (2012). Difference in mother–child interaction between pre-term- and term-born preschoolers with and without disabilities. Acta Paediatrica, 101(6), 597603. doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.2012.02599.xGoogle Scholar
Lee, S.-Y., Grantham, C. H., Shelton, S., & Meaney-Delman, D. (2012). Does activity matter: an exploratory study among mothers with pre-term infants? Archives of Women's Mental Health, 15(3), 185192. doi:10.1007/s00737-012-0275-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeVine, R. A., LeVine, S., Schnell-Anzola, B., Rowe, M. L., & Dexter, E. (2012). Literacy and mothering: How women's schooling changes the lives of the world's children. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Infants Born at Risk: Behavior and Development (Book Review) (Vol. 3, pp. 287287): Elsevier Inc.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (2015). Summary and commentary from The psychology of sex differences. In V. Burr & V. Burr (Eds.), Gender and Psychology. (pp. 1842). New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Mahoney, G., Boyce, G., Fewell, R. R., Spiker, D., & Wheeden, C. A. (1998). The relationship of parent-child interaction to the effectiveness of early intervention services for at-risk children and children with disabilities. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18(1), 517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marfo, K. (1992). Corrlates of maternal directiveness with children who are developmentally delayed. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 62, 219233. doi:10.1037/h0079334CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, J., & Lewis, E. (2014). ‘It's the way you talk to them.’ The child's environment: Early Years Practitioners' perceptions of its influence on speech and language development, its assessment and environment targeted interventions. Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 30(3), 337352. doi:10.1177/0265659013516331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E. F., Flynn, V., & Eichorst, D. L. (2005). Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development. Journal of child language, 32, 6391. doi:10.1017/S0305000904006634CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menyuk, P., Liebergott, J. W., & Schultz, M. C. (1995). Early Language Development in Full-term and Premature infants. Hoboken: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Minde, K. K., Perrotta, M., & Marton, P. (1985). Maternal caretaking and play with full-term and premature infants. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 26(2), 231244. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1985.tb02262.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M., & Simons, A. D. (1991). Diathesis-Stress Theories in the Context of Life Stress Research: Implications for the Depressive Disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 406425. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A., & Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother–child interactions. Early Human Development, 79(2), 145158. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newnham, C. A., Milgrom, J., & Skouteris, H. (2009). Effectiveness of a Modified Mother–Infant Transaction Program on Outcomes for Pre-term Infants from 3 to 24 months of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 1726. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.09.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nordhov, S. M., Rønning, J. A., Dahl, L. B., Ulvund, S. E., Tunby, J., & Kaaresen, P. I. (2010). Early intervention improves cognitive outcomes for pre-term infants: Randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 126(5), e1088-e1094. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-0778CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obradovic, J., Bush, N. R., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N. E., & Boyce, W. T. (2010). Biological Sensitivity to Context: The Interactive Effects of Stress Reactivity and Family Adversity on Socioemotional Behavior and School Readiness. Child Development, 81(1), 270289. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01394.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paavola, L., Kunnari, S., & Moilanen, I. (2005). Maternal responsiveness and infant intentional communication: implications for the early communicative and linguistic development. Child: Care, Health and Development, 31, 727735. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2005.00566.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rahkonen, P., Heinonen, K., Pesonen, A. K., Lano, A., Autti, T., Puosi, R., Huhtala, E., Andersson, S., Metsaranta, M., & Räikkönen, K. (2014). Mother-child interaction is associated with neurocognitive outcome in extremely low gestational age children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 55(4), 311318. doi:10.1111/sjop.12133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, S., Wake, M., Ukoumunne, O. C., Bavin, E., Prior, M., Cini, E., Conway, L., Eadie, P., & Bretherton, L. (2010). Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: Findings from early language in Victoria study. Pediatrics, 126(6), e1530e1537. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-0254CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185205. doi:10.1017/S0305000907008343CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudy, D., & Grusec, J. E. (2001). Correlates of authoritarian parenting in individualist and collectivist cultures and implications for understanding the transmission of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(2), 202212. doi:10.1177/0022022101032002007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saigal, S., & Doyle, L. W. (2008). Pre-term birth 3 – An overview of mortality and sequelae of pre-term birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet, 371(9608), 261269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., Savini, S., Broccoli, S., Justice, L., Alessandroni, R., & Faldella, G. (2011). Longitudinal trajectories of gestural and linguistic abilities in very pre-term infants in the second year of life. Neuropsychologia, 49(13), 36773688. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigel, I. E., & McGillicuddy-Delisi, A. V. (2002). Parent beliefs are cognitions: the dynamic belief systems model. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (pp. 485508): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., & Lehtonen, L. (2009). The early lexical development and its predictive value to language skills at 2 years in very-low-birth-weight children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42(2), 107123. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stolt, S., Korja, R., Matomaki, J., Lapinleimu, H., Haataja, L., & Lehtonen, L. (2014). Early relations between language development and the quality of mother-child interaction in very-low-birth-weight children. Early Human Development, 90, 219225. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.02.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swaroop, J., Nanda, P., & Kang, T. K. (2001). Perceptual ability as correlate of age, sex and locale. Psycho-Lingua, 31(2), 131134Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Chen, L. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (1998). Mothers' knowledge about children's play and language development: Short-term stability and interrelations. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 115124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 12451260. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, L. J., Clark, C. A. C., Pritchard, V. E., Anderson, P. J., & Inder, T. E. (2011). Neonatal White Matter Abnormalities Predict Global Executive Function Impairment in Children Born Very Pre-term. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(1), 2241. doi:10.1080/87565641.2011.540530CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wulbert, M. (1975). Language Delay and Associated Mother-Child Interactions. Developmental Psychology, 11(1), 61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Younesian, S., Sullivan, K. A., Gilmore, L., & Yadegari, F. (2018). Reliability and Factor Structure of the Maternal Interactive Beliefs Questionnaire Translated into Persian. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 0265659018780944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zegiob, L. E., Arnold, S., & Forehand, R. (1975). An examination of observer effects in parent-child interaction. Child Development, 46 (2), 509512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Rice, M. L., & Slegers, D. W. (2007). Late Language Emergence at 24 Months: An Epidemiological Study of Prevalence, Predictors, and Covariates. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(6), 15621592. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/106)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed