Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nmvwc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-30T20:17:19.122Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Information level and young children's phonological accuracy*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Lisa Goffman
Affiliation:
Purdue University
Richard G. Schwartz*
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Klara Marton
Affiliation:
City University of New York
*
Address for correspondence: Program in Speech and Hearing Sciences, Graduate School and University Center, City University of New York, 33 West 42 Street, New York, NY 10036, USA, e-mail rschwartz@gc.cuny.edu.

Abstract

The influence of information level on the production accuracy of 20 children (22 to 28 months) was examined. The data were children's productions of nouns in sets of utterances referring to triplets of pictures representing noun-verb-noun utterances. In each triplet one noun remained the same, thus decreasing in information value from the first to the third picture (new, moderately old, and old information). Words representing new information were produced more accurately than words representing old information. The types of errors did not differ. Further evidence of this effect was provided by an examination of the duration of new versus old word productions by 12 of the children. Productions encoding new information were consistently longer on average than those encoding old information. The results provide experimental evidence of an effect observed in adults that indicates early sensitivity to information level. However, because of the children's young age, the effects are characterized as a speaker-internal process that only serendipitously corresponds to listener needs.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Support was provided by Grant RO1 DC 00583–05, Input-output relationships in speech and language impairments, from the National Institute of Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. This research was part of a thesis conducted by the first author under the direction of the second author at Purdue University. George Allen and Ronnie Wilbur provided valuable suggestions as thesis committee members. Karen Pollock's assistance in reliability assessments and her discussions with the authors on these topics are gratefully acknowledged.

References

REFERENCES

Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language 6, 183200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology 8, 521–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1958). Stress and information. American Speech 33, 520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1963). Length, vowel, juncture. Linguistics I, 529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1981). Two kinds of vowels, two kinds of rhythm. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Campbell, T. & Shriberg, L. (1982). Associations among pragmatic factors, linguistic stress, and natural phonological processes in speech-delayed children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 25, 547–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. (1974). Language and consciousness. Language 50, 111–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, C. (1988). Differential shortening of repeated content words produced in various communicative contexts. Language and Speech 31, 307–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, C. & Housum, J. (1987). Talkers' signalling of ‘new’ and ‘old’ words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory and Language 26, 489504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, T. (1977). Revision behaviours in the speech of normal children developing language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 20, 303–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. (1979). Informativeness, presupposition, and semantic choice in single word utterances. In Ochs, E. and Schieffelin, B. (eds), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hedrick, D., Prather, E. & Tobin, A. (1975). Sequenced inventory of communication development. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Hornby, P. & Hass, W. (1970). Use of contrastive stress by preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 13, 395–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunnicut, S. (1985). Intelligibility versus redundancy – conditions of dependency. Language and Speech 28, 4556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, D. (1981). Procedures for analysis of phonological ability in child language. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Schwartz, R., Folger, M., Newhoff, M. & Wilcox, M. (1979). Children's imitations of lexical items. Child Development 50, 1927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, P. (1963). Some effects of semantic and grammatical context on the production and perception of speech. Language and Speech 6, 172–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (1978). Sentential devices for conveying givenness and newness: a cross cultural developmental study. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 17, 539–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38 (Serial No. 149).Google Scholar
Schwartz, R. G. & Goffman, L. (1995). Metrical patterns of words and production accuracy. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 876–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, R., Wilcox, M. & Folger, M. (1979). Strategies for dealing with communicative failure during early language development. Paper presented to the American Speech and Hearing Association.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. & Lorge, I. (1952). The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Teacher's College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Wieman, L. (1976). Stress patterns of early child language. Journal of Child Language 3, 283–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar