Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-n9wrp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T03:11:16.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The importance of modeling pragmatic syntactic bootstrapping

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2023

Toben H. MINTZ*
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychology and Linguistics, University of Southern California, USA
*

Extract

Syntactic bootstrapping is based on the premise that there are probabilistic correspondences between the syntactic structure in which a word occurs and the word’s meaning, and that such links hold, with some degree of generality, cross-linguistically. The procedure has been extensively discussed with respect to verbs, where it has been proposed as a mechanism for constraining the massive ambiguity that arises when inferring the meaning of a verb that is used to describe an event (Fisher, Hall, Rakowitz & Gleitman, 1994; Gleitman, 1990; Gleitman, Cassidy, Nappa, Papafragou & Trueswell, 2005). In her keynote paper (Hacquard, 2022), Hacquard focuses on classes of verbs for which inferences about meaning are arguably even harder, because they involve concepts that have no observable counterparts: these are attitude verbs such as think and want, and modals such as must and can. She walks us through, in meticulous detail, the limits of a purely syntactic bootstrapping mechanism, and she describes how augmenting syntactic information with pragmatic information, via pragmatic syntactic bootstrapping (Hacquard, 2022; Hacquard & Lidz, 2019), might address these limitations. The proposal is exciting, and the detail with which Hacquard works through these examples is impressive; she supports her arguments with behavioral experiments, corpus analyses, and two very targeted computational analyses. In this commentary I suggest that Hacquard’s proposal is laid out in sufficient detail such that a comprehensive computational modeling effort would be fruitful for evaluating and further developing her account.

Type
Invited Commentary
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Fisher, C., Hall, D. G., Rakowitz, S., & Gleitman, L. R. (1994). When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth. Lingua, 92(C), 333375. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(94)90346-8Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1(1), 355. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0101_2Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A., & Trueswell, J. C. (2005). Hard Words. Language Learning and Development 1(1), 2364. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15473341LLD0101_4Google Scholar
Hacquard, V. (2022). Being pragmatic about syntactic bootstrapping. Journal of Child Language, 124. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000922000605Google Scholar
Hacquard, V., & Lidz, J. (2019). Children’s attitude problems: Bootstrapping verb meaning from syntax and pragmatics. Mind & Language, 34(1), 7396. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12192Google Scholar
Huang, N., White, A. S., Liao, C. H., Hacquard, V., & Lidz, J. (2021). Syntactic bootstrapping of attitude verbs despite impoverished morphosyntax. Language Acquisition, 29(1), 2753. https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2021.1934686Google Scholar
Yang, Y. (2022). Are you asking me or telling me? Learning clause types and speech acts in English and Mandarin. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland.Google Scholar