Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T01:47:20.525Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early grammatical development in Spanish children with Down syndrome*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 January 2013

MIGUEL GALEOTE*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Málaga, Spain
PILAR SOTO
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain
EUGENIA SEBASTIÁN
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain
ELENA CHECA
Affiliation:
Universidad de Málaga, Spain
CONCEPCIÓN SÁNCHEZ-PALACIOS
Affiliation:
Universidad de Málaga, Spain
*
Address for correspondence: Miguel Galeote, Universidad de Málaga – Developmental Psychology, Facultad de Psicología // Campus de Teatinos, s/n. Málaga 20071, Spain. e-mail: mgaleote@uma.es

Abstract

The objective of this work was to analyze morphosyntactic development in a wide sample of children with Down syndrome (DS) (n = 92) and children with typical development (TD) (n = 92) with a mental age (MA) of 20 to 29 months. Children were individually matched for gender and MA (Analysis 1) and for vocabulary size (Analysis 2). Information about morphosyntax was obtained using an adaptation of the CDI for children with DS. In both analyses, the number of children with DS and with TD who combined words was similar. Analysis 1 showed that children with DS produced shorter utterances, with less morphosyntactic complexity and less morphological suffixes than children with TD, despite having the same mental age. The developmental pattern was similar, although slower in children with DS. Analysis 2 showed that the performance of children with DS was lower than the performance of children with TD in relation to morphosyntactic complexity and morphological suffixes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by a grant from the Spanish Ministry for Science and Innovation (Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación) and by the European Regional Development Fund (Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional – FEDER; PSI2008-02748). We would like to express our thanks to the children and their families, and to the many therapists of Down syndrome associations and early intervention units, as well as to the staff and the people from the nurseries who participated in our research.

References

REFERENCES

Abbeduto, L., Murphy, M. M., Cawthon, S. W., Richmond, E. K., Weissman, M. D., Karadottir, S. & O'Brien, A. (2003). Receptive language skills of adolescents and young adults with Down or fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation 108, 149–60.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Dale, P. S. & Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds.), The handbook of child language, 96151. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Goodman, J. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes 12, 507586.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Goodman, J. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the lexicon. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The emergence of language, 2979. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznik, S., Reilly, J. & Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 21, 85124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berglund, E., Eriksson, M. & Johansson, I. (2001). Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 179–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, A., Buckley, S., MacDonald, J. & Bird, G. (1995). Investigating the literacy, language and memory skills of children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research & Practice 3, 5358.Google Scholar
Cardoso-Martins, C., Mervis, C. B. & Mervis, C. A. (1985). Early vocabulary acquisition by children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Deficiency 90, 177–84.Google ScholarPubMed
Caselli, M. C., Casadio, P. & Bates, E. (1999). A comparison of the transition from first words to grammar in English and Italian. Journal of Child Language 26, 69111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, M. C., Marchetti, M. C. & Vicari, S. (1994). Conoscenze lessicali e primo sviluppo morfosintattico. In Contardi, A. & Vicari, S. (eds.), Le persone Down, 2848. Milán: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Monaco, L., Trasciani, M. & Vicari, S. (2008). Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology 22(1), 2735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., Longobardi, E., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C. & Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41, 1125–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, R. S. (1995). Language development in children and adolescents with Down Syndrome. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds.), The handbook of child language, 641–63. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. S., Hesketh, L. J. & Kistler, D. J. (2002). Predicting longitudinal change in language production and comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: hierarchical linear modelling. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45, 902915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R. S., Seung, H.-K., Schwartz, S. E. & Kay-Raining, Bird E. (1998). Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: II. Production deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41, 861–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, J. A. & Marchman, V. A. (2007). Grammar and the lexicon: developmental ordering in language acquisition. Child Development 78, 190212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eadie, P. A., Fey, M. E., Douglas, J. M. & Parsons, C. L. (2002). Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45, 720–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabbretti, D., Pizzuto, E., Vicari, S. & Volterra, V. (1997). A story description task in children with Down syndrome: lexical and morphosyntactic abilities. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities Research 41, 165–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S. & Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: user's guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fowler, A. E. (1990). Language abilities in children with Down syndrome: evidence for specific syntactic delay. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (eds.), Children with Down syndrome: a developmental perspective, 302328. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, A. E., Gelman, R. & Gleitman, L. R. (1994). The course of language learning in children with Down syndrome: longitudinal and language level comparisons with young normally developing children. In Tager-Flusberg, H. (ed.), Constraints on language acquisition: studies of atypical children, 91140. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Galeote, M., Sebastián, E., Checa, E., Rey, R. & Soto, P. (2011). The development of vocabulary in Spanish children with Down syndrome: comprehension, production and gestures. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability 36, 184–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galeote, M., Soto, P., Checa, E., Gómez, A. & Lamela, E. (2008). The acquisition of productive vocabulary in Spanish children with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability 33, 292302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grela, B. G. (2002). Lexical verb diversity in children with Down syndrome. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 16, 251–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grela, B. G. (2003). Do children with Down syndrome have difficulty with argument structure? Journal of Communication Disorders 36, 263–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M., Longobardi, E. & Casselli, M. C. (2003). Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down's syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 38, 179–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Newton, T. & Conboy, B. (2005). CDI-Inventarios MacArthur-Bates del desarrollo de habilidades comunicativas: guía del usuario y manual técnico. México: Manual Moderno.Google Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E. & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development in Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Language 20, 523–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, C. (1993). Expressive language delay in children with Down syndrome. Down's Syndrome: Research and Practice 1, 1014.Google Scholar
Josse, D. (1997). Escala de desarrollo psicomotor de la primera infancia Brunet-Lézine-Revisado [Brunet–Lézine Psychomotor Development Scale-Revised – Spanish edition]. Madrid: Psymtéc.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Gallego, C., Gallo, P., Karousou, A., Mariscal, S. & Martínez, M. (2005). Inventarios de desarrollo comunicativo MacArthur: Manual [The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Manual]. Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F. (1988). The developmental asynchrony of language development in children with Down syndrome. In Nadel, L. (ed.), The psychobiology of Down syndrome, 167–98. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. F. (1992). Development of speech and language in children with Down syndrome. In Lott, I. T. & McLoy, E. E. (eds.), Clinical care for persons with Down syndrome, 3950. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F. (1999). Profiles on language development in children with Down syndrome. In Miller, J. F., Leddy, M. & Leavitt, L. A. (eds.), Improving communication of people with Down syndrome, 1139. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. & García-Soto, X. R. (2003). El diagnóstico del desarrollo comunicativo en la primera infancia: adaptación de las escalas MacArthur al gallego [The assessment of communicative development in the early years: adaptation of the MacArthur scales to Galician]. Psicothema 15, 352–61.Google Scholar
Roberts, J. E., Price, J., Barnes, E., Nelson, L., Burchinal, M., Hennon, E. A., Moskowitz, L., Edwards, A., Malkin, C., Anderson, K., Misenheimer, J. & Hooper, S. R. (2007). Receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, and speech production of boys with fragile X syndrome in comparison to boys with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation 112, 177–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rondal, J. A. (1978). Developmental sentence scoring procedure and the delay-difference question in language development of Down's syndrome children. Mental Retardation 16, 169–71.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1993). Down' syndrome. In Bishop, D. & Mogford, K. (eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances, 165–76. Hove: LEA.Google Scholar
Rutter, T. & Buckley, S. (1994). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in children with Down's syndrome. Down's Syndrome: Research and Practice 2(2), 7682.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H., Rescorla, L., Tager-Flusberg, H., Fowler, A. & Sudhalter, V. (1991). The relation of utterance length to grammatical complexity in normal and language-disordered groups. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 2345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer Harris, N. G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W. & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental Neuropsychology 13, 345–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1, 1157–256. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E. T., Chapman, R. & Wagner, L. (2002). Complex sentence production by adolescents with Down syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics 23, 163–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S., Caselli, M. C. & Tonucci, F. (2000). Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia 38, 634–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Caselli, M. C., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F. & Volterra, V. (2002). Language acquisition in special populations: a comparison between Down and Williams syndromes. Neuropsychologia 40, 2461–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoder, P. J. & Warren, S. F. (2004). Early predictors of language in children with and without Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation 109, 285300.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zampini, L. & D'Odorico, L. (2011). Lexical and syntactic development in Italian children with Down's syndrome. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 46, 386–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed