Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vpsfw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T12:10:02.512Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Context-specificity and generalization in the acquisition of pronominal distinctions*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Shulamuth Chiat
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science

Abstract

The pronoun system provides a fruitful area for investigating the conditions under which children make linguistic generalizations. Pronouns are defined by a complex of semantic, syntactic, and morphological distinctions whose interaction is only partially consistent. In the course of acquiring them, children often make systematic errors which reflect novel generalizations from the adult input. A distributional analysis was applied to the errors made by 48 children in marking distinctions of person, possession, and case in their spontaneous use of pronouns. The analysis indicated that children do not make maximal generalizations which extend a particular feature to all related contexts. Rather, they acquire specific complexes of features, and are quite conservative in extrapolating from one feature complex to another.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This paper is based on research undertaken for a Ph.D. under the supervision of Dr N. V. Smith at University College London, and with the financial support of the Social Science Research Council of Great Britain. Address for correspondence: School for the Study of Disorders of Human Communication, 86 Blackfriars Road, London SE1 8HA.

References

REFERENCES

Bellugi, U. (1971). Simplification in children's language. In Huxley, R. & Ingram, E. (eds), Language acquisition: models and methods. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J. & Miller, G. A. (eds), Linguistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Charney, R. (1979). Speech roles and the development of personal pronouns. JChLang 7. 3.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1977). From gesture to word: on the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In Bruner, J. S. & Garton, A.. (eds), Human growth and development: Wolfson College lectures 1976. Oxford: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Cooley, C. H. (1908). A study of the early use of self-words by a child. PsychRev 15. 339–57.Google Scholar
Deutsch, W. & Pechmann, T. (1978). Ihr, dir, or mir? On the acquisition of pronouns in German children. Cognition 6. 155–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huxley, R. (1970). The development of the correct use of subject personal pronouns in two children. In d'Arcais, G. B. Flores & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds), Advances in psycho-linguistics. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1922). Language: its nature, development, and origin. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Kaper, W. (1976). Pronominal case-errors. JChLang 3. 439–41.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. (1979). How to get from words to sentences. In Aaronson, D. & Rieber, R. (eds), Psycholinguistic research: implications and applications. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shipley, E. F. & Shipley, T. E. (1969). Quaker children's use of Thee: a relational analysis. JVLVB 8. 112–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanz, C. (1974). Cognitive principles underlying children's errors in pronominal case-marking. JChLang 1. 271–6.Google Scholar