Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gvh9x Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T19:36:09.111Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From one word to two words: repetition patterns on the way to structured speech*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Edy Veneziano*
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Hermine Sinclair
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Ioanna Berthoud
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
*
F.P.S.E., Université de Genève, 1211 Genève 4, Switzerland.

Abstract

This paper gives an account of the transition from one-word to multiword utterances based on the productions of one child from age 1;5.23 to 1;8.15 in spontaneous interaction with her mother. The authors' interpretation of the observed development emphasizes: (1) the initial dissociation and later co-ordination of temporal chaining of elements on the one hand and meaning-relatedness between elements on the other; and (2) the psychological importance of repetition patterns for the change from single-word functioning to meaning-related and temporally-chained multi-word utterances, i.e. utterances that show the duality of patterning characteristic of human language.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by a grant from the Fonds National suisse de la Recherche Scientifique (FNRS) (No. 1. 168–0.85) to Hermine Sinclair and loanna Berthoud. We wish to express our thanks to the parents and children who participated in this study.

References

REFERENCES

Antinucci, F. & Parisi, D. (1973). Early language acquisition: a model and some data. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds). Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, M. (1979). Prerequisites for reference. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds), Developmental Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Atkinson, M. (1985). The status of children's single-word speech. In Barrett, M. D. (ed.), Children's single-word speech. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge MA.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1963). The ontogeny of English phrase structure: the first phase. Language 39. 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41. No. 164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, G. (1979). Some reasons why single-word utterances are not. Journal of Child Language 6. 411–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolitsky, M. (1983). The birth of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 12. 353–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dore, J., Franklin, M. B., Miller, R. & Ramer, A. L. H. (1976). Transitional phenomena in early language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 3. 1328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbers, L. (1982). Operating principles in repetitive babbling: a cognitive continuity approach. Cognition 17. 4563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. N. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kempen, G. & Hoenkamp, E. (1987). An incremental procedural grammar for sentence formulation. Cognitive Science 11. 201–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E. V. M. (1984). Interactional style, and children's language learning. Topics in Language Disorders 4. 1523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics. Vol. 1. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Martinet, A. (1949). La double articulation linguistique. Travaux du Cercle Linguistique de Copenhague 5. 30–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R. & Ervin, S. (1964). The development of grammar in child language. In U. Bellugi & R. Brown (eds), The acquisition of language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 29. No. 92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E., Schieffelin, B. B. & Platt, M. L. (1979). Propositions across utterances and speakers. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds), Developmental Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Saussure, F. de (1916). Cours de linguistique générale. Edition critique by Tullio de Mauro. Paris: Payot.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1974). Relational concepts underlying language. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Lloyd, L. (eds), Language perspectives: acquisition, retardation and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. (1974). The child's language from one to two: the origins of construction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hawaii, Honolulu.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. (1979). A real early stage: an unzippered condensation of a dissertation on child language. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds), Developmental Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978). The relationship between cognitive processes and the development of communication skills. In Keasey, C. B. (ed.), Nebraska Symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Sinclair, H. (1988). Structures de l'intelligence et capacités communicatives. In Cornoldi, C. & Vianello, R. (eds), Handicap, comunicazione e linguaggio. Bergamo: Edizioni Juvenilia.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (1988). Vocal-verbal interaction and the construction of early lexical knowledge. In Smith, M. & Locke, J. (eds), The emergent lexicon: the child's development of a linguistic vocabulary. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar