Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pjpqr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-25T03:05:41.930Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Experiments with the soluble antigen of rabies in suckling mouse brains

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2009

M. van den Ende
Affiliation:
The C.S.I.R. and U.C.T. Virus Research Unit, Department of Pathology, University of Cape Town (South Africa)
A. Polson
Affiliation:
The C.S.I.R. and U.C.T. Virus Research Unit, Department of Pathology, University of Cape Town (South Africa)
G. S. Turner
Affiliation:
The C.S.I.R. and U.C.T. Virus Research Unit, Department of Pathology, University of Cape Town (South Africa)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

A study has been made of the properties of soluble antigen in the brains of infant mice infected intracerebrally with the Flury strain of rabies virus.

Soluble antigen is produced at the same time as infective virus, and reaches a high concentration in a period of 2–3 days.

It can be partially purified by precipitation at pH 4·3. It is partially resistant to the action of trypsin, RNAse and DNAse. It is relatively stable at pH 6–10.

Experimental results suggest that the soluble antigen remains antigenically active after heating at 56° C. and treatment with 0·5% phenol or 0·35% formal-dehyde, but that such heating markedly reduces the ability to stimulate formation of neutralizing antibody.

Rabbits and mice appear to differ in the production of neutralizing antibody following immunization against soluble antigen in which residual live virus was inactivated by heat, phenol or formaldehyde.

It is suggested that this difference may depend on the different susceptibility to traces of incompletely inactivated virus remaining in the immunizing antigens.

The authors are grateful to Miss T. Madsen for her assistance in some aspects of this work. Dr N. Sapeika kindly made available facilities for the in vitro anaphylaxis experiments.

Financial assistance was received from the Nkana-Kitwe and Chingola Poliomyelitis Research Funds.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1957

References

Ada, G. L., Perky, B. T. & Pye, J. (1953). Aust. J. exp. Biol. meal. Sci. 31, 391404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casals, J. (1949). Proc. Soc. exp. Biol., N.Y., 70, 339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casals, J. & Olitsky, P. K. (1950). Proc. Soc. exp. Biol., N.Y., 75, 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mead, T. H. (1956). Personal communication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moulton, J. E. (1954). Amer. J. Path. 30, 533.Google Scholar
Oakley, C. L. & Fulthorpe, A. J. (1953). J. Path. Bact. 65, 49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polson, A. (1956). Biochim. biophys. Acta, 22, 61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polson, A., Joubert, J. J. & Haig, D. A. (1946). Biochem. J. 40, 265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polson, A. & Wessels, P. (1953). Proc. Soc. exp. Biol., N.Y., 84, 317–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, L. J. & Muench, H. (1938). Amer. J. Hyg. 27, 493.Google Scholar
Ozawa, Y. (1954). Personal communication.Google Scholar