Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T03:28:39.717Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Programming the next generation of prenatal programming of stress research: A review and suggestions for the future of the field

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2024

Nicole R. Bush*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Center for Health and Community, Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Nicole R. Bush; Email: nicole.bush@ucsf.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this article, I highlight core ideas, empirical findings, and advances in the study of how stress during pregnancy may prenatally program child neurodevelopmental, psychopathological, and health outcomes, emphasizing reviews, metanalyses, and recent contributions of conceptual and empirical work. The article offers a perspective on the history of this area of science, the underrecognized contributions of influential scholars from diverse fields of study, what we know from the evidence to date, the persistent challenges in sorting through what is left to learn, and suggestions for future research. I include sections focused on promoting resilience, pregnancy interventions that demonstrate positive effects across two generations, and the translational implications of the accruing data for practice and policy, highlighting opportunities for integrating across a range of fields and sectors. In the concluding sections, I discuss lessons learned from conducting this work and provide a closing summary of progress and future directions. The goal of this writing was to provide a viewpoint on some ways that emerging intergenerational transmission scholars might responsibly contribute to the future of the field of developmental psychopathology.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

The perception that women’s social and emotional experiences during pregnancy might affect the development of the child within her belly has been described across a range of ancient historical texts, and likely existed prior to recorded history. References to “prenatal programming” of child development, although not explicitly addressing the concept as understood in modern scientific terms, have appeared in ancient Hindu scriptures (Vedas; ∼500 BCE), writings of Greek physician Hippocrates (4th century BCE; Ferreira, Reference Ferreira1965), and in the wisdom shared from generation to generation by birthing people and those that care for them. Despite this long history, empirical study of the role of maternal experience on fetal development can be traced back less than a century (Sontag & Richards, Reference Sontag and Richards1938), and the topic was not truly considered by many in the fields of psychology or medicine until relatively recently. Despite comprehensive training in developmental psychopathology, and an emerging expertise in the manner in which social experiences “get under the skin” to affect development beginning early in life (Bush & Boyce, Reference Bush, Boyce, Lewis and Rudolph2014; Bush et al., Reference Bush, Obradovic, Adler and Boyce2011; Hertzman, Reference Hertzman1998), I was surprised when I first learned that this process began within the pregnancy period. Thinking with a developmental lens, and building upon my understanding of the growing experimental animal literature in this space, the logic of pursuing this science to advance understanding of the etiology of child developmental outcomes was immediately clear. However, when I first proposed to conduct research in this area as a postdoctoral fellow, the pediatricians and child psychologists that I initially consulted with were unfamiliar with the concept, which was based in epidemiologic findings of birthweight associations with adult cardiovascular outcomes (Barker, Reference Barker1990). Since then, research examining associations between prenatal maternal stress (and related factors such as depression, anxiety, and biological correlates of stress) and children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes has increased dramatically (see for review Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019; Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020). A search of publications in the PsycInfo database with “prenatal programming” in the title or abstract yields 422 articles that begin occurring in 1977, two-thirds of which have been published in the past decade. The construct is now a foundational driver for the science of two major U.S. child health research consortiums (https://www.nih.gov/research-training/environmental-influences-child-health-outcomes-echo-program and https://hbcdstudy.org/), and the topic now occupies keynote lectures across disciplines. The idea that stress in one generation can affect child health in the next, beginning in utero, has also graced mainstream media magazine covers. Prenatal programing has transitioned from a niche idea to a key construct of child health and development, and the future of developmental psychopathology should most certainly include expansion and evolution of this science.

State of the science

Studying adversity/stress exposures is a public health imperative

Beyond the value of understanding these phenomena because of their inherently interesting nature, what child psychology scholar wouldn't want to better understand all manners by which, and when, social factors affect our mental and developmental health? Deeper attention to the phenomena is necessary because of the disturbing prominence of social adversity during sensitive periods of development. A meta-analysis of 206 studies from 22 countries and more than half a million adults found that 60% reported at least one major adverse childhood experience, with 16% having 4 or more and high rates of exposure most common in individuals from low-income or minoritized racial and ethnic groups (Madigan et al., Reference Madigan, Deneault, Racine, Park, Thiemann, Zhu, Dimitropoulos, Williamson, Fearon, Cénat, McDonald, Devereux and Neville2023). In the United States, more than half of women report a history of exposure to adversity during their childhoods, which includes physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction (Frankenberger et al., Reference Frankenberger, Clements-Nolle and Yang2015; Mersky & Janczewski, Reference Mersky and Janczewski2018). In pregnancy, women’s rate of exposure to major adversities is also unfortunately high--according to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Pregnancy Risk Assessment Monitoring System, in 2010, nearly three-quarters of pregnant women experienced at least one stressful life event in the year prior to their child’s birth (Burns et al., Reference Burns, Farr and Howards2015), such as a family member being hospitalized/dying, separation from a partner, and job loss. Interpersonal violence is also a key concern, with the World Health Organization estimating that one-third of women are subjected to intimate partner violence during their lifetime, with up to 13% facing intimate partner violence during pregnancy (Organization, 2021). Notably, women disproportionally experience these stressors – especially those from lower income families and communities of color (Conradt et al., Reference Conradt, Carter and Crowell2020; Stockman et al., Reference Stockman, Hayashi and Campbell2014). These high rates of exposure to violence and adversity during childhood and pregnancy reflect some of the principal antecedent threats to individual well-being and, as such, represent a pressing social issue nationally and globally. It is especially important to understand their additional deleterious health consequences on future generations.

Developmental origins of health and disease

A range of frameworks exist for examining the intergenerational impact of maternal stress exposures on offspring health. The most well-known is the concept of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD; Barker, Reference Barker2007), which posits that maternal factors during pregnancy, including experiences of stress, can have long-term implications for her child’s health. Since its origins in studies of nutritional deficiencies during pregnancy (Barker, Reference Barker1990), the theory has expanded to include understanding of the various maternal-placental-fetal biological pathways through which social experiences can affect offspring development (see for reviews Entringer et al., Reference Entringer, Buss and Wadhwa2015; Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019; Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020). This work draws from evolutionary biology and developmental plasticity frameworks (Ellis & Del Giudice, Reference Ellis and Del Giudice2014; Gluckman et al., Reference Gluckman, Low, Buklijas, Hanson and Beedle2011; Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2011) wherein environmental conditions are understood to have shaped an organism’s survival, such as differences in the availability of safety from predators, shelter, nutrient rich foods, and other challenges that might impact an organism’s bodily integrity, functioning, and reproductive fitness. Stress biology is considered to be a primary mediator of the effects of these conditions, and cross-species research has demonstrated that biological markers of stress activation (evidence is predominantly from study of glucocorticoids), are transmitted from the mother to the fetus throughout pregnancy in a manner that directly or indirectly can impact the development of offspring bodily structures and physiological systems (Glover et al., Reference Glover, O'Donnell, O'Connor and Fisher2018; Howland et al., Reference Howland, Sandman and Glynn2017; Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019). Thus, maternal prenatal stress was identified as an important aspect of the intrauterine environment with the capacity to influence a broad range of offspring developmental outcomes. The fetal period, with its exceptionally rapid pace of development, provides a particularly sensitive window for intrauterine inputs, such as maternal circulating stress hormones and immunologic activity. This sensitivity is key to survival outside the womb, as it allows tailoring and adaptation to changing inputs (Entringer et al., Reference Entringer, Buss and Wadhwa2015) in a manner that can have positive and negative implications for development and health across the lifecourse (Davis & Narayan, Reference Davis and Narayan2020).

Evidence for maternal stress during pregnancy

A robust body of rapidly accumulating empirical evidence now demonstrates the myriad ways in which maternal prenatal stressors or experiences of distress are associated with variability in offspring outcomes. It has been found to associate with variability in fetal and infant stress responses across multiple levels of biological and behavioral functioning (see for review DiPietro, Reference DiPietro2004; Entringer et al., Reference Entringer, Buss and Wadhwa2015; Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019). The evidence for intergenerational transmission of stress effects extends to outcomes across development, suggesting persistence of effects into early childhood and adolescence in the form of socioemotional and behavioral problems as well as a full range of more specific indicators of neurodevelopment, cognitive development, negative affectivity, difficult temperament and psychiatric disorders (Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020). Using externalizing problems as an example, a recent meta-analytic review of 55 studies (Tung et al., Reference Tung, Hipwell, Grosse, Battaglia, Cannova, English, Quick, Llamas, Taylor and Foust2023) found that psychological distress during pregnancy was uniquely associated with increased risk for child externalizing problems (age 2–18 years), including aggressive, disinhibited, and impulsive behaviors. Notably, these effects were largely similar across the range of externalizing outcomes examined, although the size of effects was larger for aggressive behaviors than nonaggressive rule-breaking behaviors. In addition, Tung et al. found evidence for persistent small effects from early childhood through adolescence, suggesting they occur across developmental periods. Although such overviews of findings are suggestive, limitations in study designs across all types of psychopathology outcomes make it difficult to discern whether effects arise early in life and are maintained, may reveal themselves for some subsets of children only at later period of development, or occur with some combination of timing effects

Notably, very few studies have examined prenatal stress effects into adulthood. In one exception, a very small sample using the naturalistic experiment of a severe ice storm found associations between maternal pregnancy exposure and broad autism phenotype in offspring during young adulthood (Li et al., Reference Li, Laplante, Elgbeili and King2023). In a larger sample, extensions of the Dutch Famine Study into late adulthood (mean age 73 years) continue to find effects of adversity in pregnancy and psychological health at these older ages (Hilberdink et al., Reference Hilberdink, Olff, Roseboom, de Rooij and van Zuiden2023). Although multidecade follow-up is still rare, the findings published to date suggest persistence of risk for many individuals. Collectively, the data spanning small studies with deep phenotyping, larger epidemiological studies with more limited measurement quality but greater power to examine small and across-cohorts consistency of effects, and quasi-experimental designs, point to the same conclusions that prenatal stress associates with offspring outcomes.

Origins of intergenerational stress-transmission work

It is important to note that the majority of empirical work within the field of study of intergenerational transmission of stress effects on child health (regrettably, my own team’s included) typically fails to acknowledge the breadth of theories and thought leaders that have informed the field, particularly in the context of considering the impact of historical traumas such as the Holocaust, slavery, and colonization. This is, in part, likely due to the disparate areas of study, professional societies, and journals the experts have published in, or because graduate training and mentorship has not highlighted these cross-over works. However, in the current era of comprehensive internet search capacity and rightfully increased demand for inclusive scholarship, it is imperative that we better consider and acknowledge key figures whose work contributes to understanding the intergenerational impacts of stress in these historical contexts. It is worth noting that many of the strongest voices in this space are women with lived experience as mothers and birthing supporters and their wisdom may not be accepted or amplified as robustly as that of the father of “the Barker Hypothesis” (Barker, Reference Barker1990) due to the marginalization of women and their voices in academia and centers of power and knowledge, more broadly. These thought leaders have played significant roles in shaping our understanding of intergenerational transmission of stress effects on child health, particularly in the context of historical structural harm inflicted upon marginalized groups, e.g., slavery, colonization. For example, focusing on the historical trauma experienced by Indigenous populations, Dr Maria Yellow Horse Brave Heart developed the concept of “historical unresolved grief” to explain the intergenerational impact of settler colonialism and forced assimilation on the mental health of Native American communities (Heart, Reference Heart1999), which scholars continue to advance within multilevel frameworks to address individual, family, and community impacts (see for review Evans-Campbell, Reference Evans-Campbell2008). Dr Rachel Yehuda and colleagues, who appear to have brought the concept of intergenerational trauma to the psychiatric literature (see Yehuda et al., Reference Yehuda, Bell, Bierer and Schmeidler2008), have extensively studied the effects of trauma on Holocaust survivors and their descendants, exploring the transmission of trauma-related stress and its impact on mental health across generations (Dashorst et al., Reference Dashorst, Mooren, Kleber, de Jong and Huntjens2019). Dr Joy DeGruy, in her book “Post Traumatic Slave Syndrome,” (Degruy-Leary, Reference Degruy-Leary2017) describes the intergenerational effects of centuries of slavery on African American communities, discussing how historical trauma continues to shape the psychological and physical health of descendants. Dr Arline Geronimus, who developed the “weathering hypothesis” to describe the process by which repeated stress activation in response to socially-structured stressors can contribute to risk of disease in marginalized groups, has highlighted the value of considering these processes across the life course, using a DOHaD framework (Geronimus, Reference Geronimus2013) and recently reminded us of the importance of “centering on the margins” to prioritize the perspectives about stress during pregnancy provided by the populations at highest risk of concerning outcomes, and considering maternal-specific weathering (Geronimus et al., Reference Geronimus, Bound and Hughes2023). These examples provide just a small set of examples of an important body of work that contributes to ongoing discussions on resilience, healing, and the development of effective interventions, and we should endeavor to integrate it more fully within our work.

Evidence for maternal preconception stress/childhood adversity

Although the bulk of DOHaD science has focused on pregnancy exposures, and society is increasingly aware of those potential effects on offspring, the concept of preconception adversity effects crossing generations is rarely considered and remains poorly understood. From a life course development framework, however, childhood and pregnancy are each sensitive periods for women during which exposure to stressors may affect women’s bodies and behavior in a manner that can impact offspring development and health. Indeed, an emerging line of investigation, leveraging insights from experimental animal research, has found that exposure to traumatic events during one’s own childhood predicts one’s health and well-being but also that of the offspring, with greater adversity associated with increased risk for early birth (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Gotman and Yonkers2016) and psychopathology (Schickedanz et al., Reference Schickedanz, Halfon, Sastry and Chung2018). Systematic review of this newer literature found, across 12 studies, consistent positive associations between maternal childhood adversity and their child’s internalizing problems (Rowell & Neal-Barnett, Reference Rowell and Neal-Barnett2022), although the authors highlighted limitations of that work, including underrepresentation of low-income families and families of color, and that internalizing was assessed entirely via parent report. Meta-analysis of 12 studies (Su et al., Reference Su, D’Arcy and Meng2022) has also shown that maternal childhood maltreatment has small effect on offspring depression and internalizing, with stronger effects for children of color.

Multiple periods of maternal adversity

A few teams have considered preconception and pregnancy distress simultaneously to discern independence. One study using a biomarker of HPA axis functioning during pregnancy (Thomas-Argyriou et al., Reference Thomas-Argyriou, Letourneau, Dewey, Campbell and Giesbrecht2021) and another that assessed maternal anxiety and depression during the perinatal period (Letourneau et al., Reference Letourneau, Dewey, Kaplan, Ntanda, Novick, Thomas, Deane, Leung, Pon, Giesbrecht and Team2019) found independent contributions of adversity at each period with parent-reported early childhood internalizing symptoms. Our work in the national ECHO PATHWAYS 3-cohort consortium confirmed that maternal exposure to childhood trauma and pregnancy adversities were independently associated with children’s total behavioral and mental health problems beyond the preschool period both continuous dimensions of psychopathology symptoms as well as greater odds of having mental health problems at or above borderline or clinical thresholds (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Rudd, Ahmad, Loftus, Swan, Nguyen, Barrett, Tylavsky, Mason, Karr, Sathyanarayana and LeWinn2023). Findings suggest that maternal childhood exposure effects on offspring are not merely operating through greater risk for adverse exposures during pregnancy. Notably, our group found maternal childhood trauma did not predict children’s self-report of anxiety and depression at age 8 (Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Ashby, Richardson, Ehmer, Scott, Dardar, Marshall and Talmi2023), suggesting that maternal childhood trauma effects may not persist to that age, at least in terms of youth-report of internalizing, although we did continue to find prenatal stress effects on continuous and clinical threshold anxiety. That said, the sum of the limited evidence suggests that both periods of maternal trauma/adversity exposure can lead to distinct, clinically meaningful impacts on children.

Some literature related to potential mechanisms for these maternal childhood-specific effects has accumulated as well. For example, our findings from a rigorous study of a small sample of women with a history of considerable adversity exposure across their lifetimes revealed that adversity exposures during childhood and pregnancy uniquely predicted maternal immune responses in adulthood (Aschbacher et al., Reference Aschbacher, Hagan, Steine, Rivera, Cole, Baccarella, Epel, Lieberman and Bush2021), providing some evidence of sustained impact on immune functioning of relevance for fetal gestation. Evidence from our larger cohort study showed maternal history of childhood trauma exposure, but not stressful events during pregnancy, predicted variation in the pregnancy hormone pCRH (Steine et al., Reference Steine, LeWinn, Lisha, Tylavsky, Smith, Bowman, Sathyanarayana, Karr, Smith, Kobor and Bush2020), which has critical roles in fetal development and birth timing and is related to both offspring neurodevelopmental outcomes and maternal psychopathology. Thinking developmentally, it is clear that experiences decades prior carry forward in bodies, and future research into the processes by which mothers’ own childhood experiences affect their reproductive physiology will be important for rounding out understanding.

Broad adoption of these newer findings is delayed in our field though. For example, in what appears to be the first systematic review and meta-analysis of mediators of the link between maternal childhood adversity and children’s socioemotional and behavioral development (Ma et al., Reference Ma, Biaggi, Sacchi, Lawrence, Chen, Pollard, Matter, Mackes, Hazelgrove, Morgan, Harding, Simonelli, Schumann, Pariante, Mehta, Montana, Rodriguez-Mateos, Nosarti and Dazzan2022), there is discussion of some evidence for antenatal or lifetime maternal depression and maternal perinatal health (e.g., gestational diabetes, abortion history and smoking during pregnancy) as mediators, but most associations reviewed in that paper well null, and prenatal programing was not mentioned/considered as a mediating process by the authors. Beyond the focus on children’s mental health outcomes, however, scholars are paying increasing attention. Our examination within the ECHO national consortium sample, drawn from 21 cohorts, found that children born to mothers with histories of maltreatment had greater risk for a range of negative child health outcomes, including clinical levels of internalizing problems, autism spectrum disorder, asthma, and obesity, as well as higher risk of multimorbidity, with patterns of associations suggesting a dose-response relationship as the count of types of maltreatment exposure increased (Moog et al., Reference Moog, Cummings, Jackson, Aschner, Barrett, Bastain, Blackwell, Bosquet Enlow, Breton, Bush, Deoni, Duarte, Ferrara, Grant, Hipwell, Jones, Leve, Lovinsky-Desir, Miller, Monk, Oken, Posner, Schmidt, Wright, Entringer, Simhan, Wadhwa, O'Connor, Musci and Buss2023). Further, a recent systematic review of 19 studies of parental ACEs and offspring health (N > 120,000; Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Ahmed, Clarke, Quinn, Beenstock and Holland2023) found negative effects on prematurity and low birthweight risk, childhood obesity, infant bronchiolitis, child brain development, cognitive delay, and a range of developmental outcomes such as expressive language, and health risk behaviors, such as childhood smoking. Although the effect sizes for each predictor are typically small for exposures, when considering their transdiagnostic impact, and that their effects early in development affect subsequent trajectories across the life course, a thoughtful focus on maternal childhood trauma exposure in the intergenerational transmission of stress literature seems quite merited.

Variability in effects

As is common in developmental psychopathology, there is considerable variability, i.e., multifinality (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996), in the associations between prenatal stress and child developmental outcomes. For example, prenatal stress may only result in differences in offspring mental health when potentiated by other risk factors. A study of adolescents born to women exposed to the violence of South African Apartheid (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Said Mohamed, Norris, Richter and Kuzawa2023) found that prenatal stress appeared to sensitize youth to the risks associated with young motherhood and household adversity. Individual differences in reactivity and regulation can greatly affect people’s susceptibility or sensitivity to context (Bush & Boyce, Reference Bush, Boyce and Cicchetti2016; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn2011), and this sensitivity to the effects of environmental exposures may occur at the maternal or fetal level. Further, perception of stress in the face of adversity, and whether it activates biological stress responses or behavioral differences relevant for fetal development matters considerably. As an illustration of this, the effects of more objective counts of major stress exposures during pregnancy on infant stress physiology have been found to only be significant for dyads where mothers reported feeling moderate to high levels of stress during their pregnancy (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Jones-Mason, Coccia, Caron, Alkon, Thomas, Coleman-Phox, Wadhwa, Laraia, Adler and Epel2017).

In addition, building upon considerations of historical trauma, other factors related to marginalized status may explain variability in intergenerational transmission of stress. For example, the insidious stressor of experiencing racism harms people across levels, from interpersonal interactions to institutional policies and cultural norms, and has been shown to be highly relevant to the quality of care pregnant women receive (McLemore et al., Reference McLemore, Altman, Cooper, Williams, Rand and Franck2018; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Criss, Kim, De La Cruz, Thai, Merchant, Hswen, Allen, Gee and Nguyen2023) and their and their children’s health outcomes within those systems of care. Clear evidence for this can be seen in health disparities for Black pregnant individuals, who are several times more likely to experience complications or death related to childbirth, and to lose their infants to death in the first year of life, compared to White individuals (Keating et al., Reference Keating, Murphey, Daily, Ryberg and Laurore2020). Higher levels of race-based discrimination have also been repeatedly found to be associated with increased risk for low birth weight and short gestational length (see for a review Heard-Garris et al., Reference Heard-Garris, Cale, Camaj, Hamati and Dominguez2018), key mediators of risk for developmental challenges later in life. Notably, however, prenatal programing constructs and biological processes are not often emphasized in work highlight intergenerational impacts of structural racism and trauma on mental health (e.g., Hankerson et al., Reference Hankerson, Moise, Wilson, Waller, Arnold, Duarte, Lugo-Candelas, Weissman, Wainberg, Yehuda and Shim2022), suggesting opportunities to build bridges between realms of science.

Intersectionality, a concept initially introduced by Kimberlé Crenshaw (Reference Crenshaw1991), emphasizes the interconnected nature of social categories such as race, gender, class, sexual orientation, disability, and other forms of identity. When applied to prenatal stress and child health, it highlights the unique and compounded challenges faced by pregnant individuals who navigate multiple intersecting identities simultaneously and may experience cumulative stressors related to multiple social categories. Prenatal stressors, such as discrimination or socioeconomic challenges, as well as access to resources, including healthcare, education, and social support, disproportionately affect certain groups and can contribute to heightened stress during pregnancy, leading to disparities in maternal health and well-being and child health outcomes. Given this, the effects of prenatal stress on child health cannot be understood in isolation but rather as part of a broader network of social structures, including systems of oppression and privilege, that influence health outcomes. The impact of intersectionality on prenatal stress effects on child health is a complex and multifaceted area ripe for study.

Although this is shifting, the majority of extant intergenerational stress-transmission work to date has focused primarily on risk factors for child psychopathology, with minimal empirical examination of factors that may buffer risk either at the level of maternal exposure or later in the environmental context of the child’s ongoing development. Findings from my teams have shown, across two different cohorts and study designs, that responsive caregiving (using rigorous observational coding) can meaningfully offset the risk for greater infant physiological stress reactivity (Jones-Mason et al., Reference Jones-Mason, Coccia, Alkon, Melanie Thomas, Laraia, Adler, Epel and Bush2023) and child behavioral-emotional problems (Ahmad et al., Reference Ahmad, Shih, LeWinn, Rivera, Graff, Mason, Karr, Sathyanarayana, Tylavsky and Bush2022) associated with prenatal stressor exposure. We’ve also found that greater levels of caregiver knowledge of infant development can buffer offspring from those risks (Ahmad et al., Reference Ahmad, Rudd, LeWinn, Mason, Murphy, Juarez, Karr, Sathyanarayana, Tylavsky and Bush2022). Understanding the resilience and coping strategies developed within communities facing intersecting challenges, and recognizing these strengths, is essential for developing effective interventions to mitigate the impact of prenatal stress on child health. Intersectionality recognizes the importance of cultural nuances in shaping experiences and outcomes within specific communities. For example, Brave Heart et al. (Reference Brave Heart, Chase, Elkins and Altschul2011) highlight how tribal cultural and regional differences may impact how the wounding across generations are experienced and addressed (2011). Although examination of potential buffers is increasing, the work in this space is relatively limited, despite its importance for identifying solutions.

Whether because of risk or protective factors, at the individual, family, or societal level, exposure to adversity is clearly not deterministic, and there is incredible heterogeneity of effects across individual people. Drawing upon insights related to ACEs screening not predicting disorder well at the level of the individual (Baldwin et al., Reference Baldwin, Caspi, Meehan, Ambler, Arseneault, Fisher, Harrington, Matthews, Odgers, Poulton, Ramrakha, Moffitt and Danese2021), one must be careful to ensure work is written and interpreted to communicate group- or population-level risk, rather than individual determinism. Just as is true for postnatal stress effects on psychopathology (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2016), prenatal stress effects also are dependent on contextual factors, outcome, timing of exposure, and outcome being considered (Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020).

Generalizability of findings across populations

Although there are important exceptions, the bulk of early empirical evidence for associations between prenatal stress and childhood mental health was developed from samples predominantly consisting of middle- to upper-income, White families drawn from countries with enormously different healthcare and social/economic systems to support families, relative to the United States (e.g., Canada, Northern Europe). This work has been incredibly valuable, and the findings that stress crosses generations to impact health is important in any context. However, the differences in populations and social settings have called into question whether such findings were generalizable to the U.S. and across subpopulations with large disparities in environmental supports and stress exposures. Notably, the evidence from more socioeconomically, racially, ethnically, and regionally diverse samples appears to align with the patterns found in samples with lower risk. For example, across our 3-cohort U.S.-based sample of sociodemographically diverse mother-child dyads from six distinct geographic regions, we found consistent patterns of findings for maternal childhood and pregnancy adversity on child mental health in early and middle childhood, in the full national sample and when the sample was restricted to various subsets of the seven data collection sites (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Rudd, Ahmad, Loftus, Swan, Nguyen, Barrett, Tylavsky, Mason, Karr, Sathyanarayana and LeWinn2023; Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Epel, Vieten, Adler, Laraia, Jones-Mason, Alkon and Bush2022). Although there are certainly differences across countries in culture, policies, and perinatal care and within countries in terms of exposure to harms and access to resources, the relative consistency of associations found across the literature suggest associations can be expected in a range of populations and are important for all societies to address.

Can we leverage intervention to positively program offspring outcomes?

Very recently, experimental and quasi-experimental evidence in humans has provided exciting evidence that positive inputs, and resultant decreases in distress or poor health during pregnancy, can lead to improvements in child outcomes. For example, our team found that infants born to women who participated in an 8-week group-based wellness intervention that reduced stress and depression showed more optimal sympathetic nervous system and behavioral regulation stress responses to a laboratory stressor than infants in the treatment as usual group (Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Epel, Vieten, Adler, Laraia, Jones-Mason, Alkon and Bush2022). A small randomized controlled trial of a nutrition+exercise intervention during pregnancy found that babies born to women in the intervention group demonstrated greater expressive language and general adaptive behavior on the Bayley at age 1, although no differences were seen for cognitive, receptive language, motor, or socioemotional outcomes (Amani et al., Reference Amani, Atkinson, Krzeczkowski, Mortaji, Schmidt and Van Lieshout2023). In addition to providing encouraging evidence for solutions, the assignment to groups and resulting “control” over some social exposures during pregnancy has led to increased confidence in the causality of those exposures. Other groups also have ongoing studies of prenatal interventions with mothers to reduce stress and improve their mental health, in an effort to document the benefits for offspring brain development, behavior, and health (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Hankin, Swales and Hoffman2018; Lugo-Candelas et al., Reference Lugo-Candelas, Talati, Glickman, Hernandez, Scorza, Monk, Kubo, Wei, Sourander and Duarte2023). Related work targeting health system-level changes to provide trauma-informed obstetric care for adolescents showed that intervention eliminated racial disparities in preterm birth and low birth weight for young Black mothers (Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Ashby, Richardson, Ehmer, Scott, Dardar, Marshall and Talmi2023), which will likely have beneficial impacts on developmental trajectories for those babies.

Some key questions remaining in prenatal stress-transmission work

Sensitive periods for exposure

One of the greatest challenges to this field is to discern potentially sensitive periods of exposure, both within the prenatal period as well as over the preconception lifetime of birthing people. One reason is because very few studies have simultaneously examined distinct effects of maternal stress exposures during and prior to pregnancy, and few have the data necessary to parse out the influence of potent stressors during a particular trimester. This is, in part, because many stress exposures are not acute nor neatly temporally delineated. For example, although divorce or separation from a coparent may occur on a given day, the conflict leading to the termination of the relationship occurred prior, and the effects post-divorce are typically pervasive and long-standing (with tremendous variability in experiences ranging from relief from interpersonal violence to distress due to loss of income and/or housing security). Even natural disasters, though their onset may be abrupt, allowing for clear determination of the initial timepoint of exposure, their maximal activation of stress levels may be somewhat acute (such as an earthquake or hurricane) but they can have consequences that can be comprehensive and long-standing (physical harm or death, loss of housing or childcare, etc.). For structural-level stressors, such as racism, which are typically pervasive and long-standing, it is nearly impossible to discern an onset of exposure to such stress in order to identify sensitive periods. That said, researchers do and should continue to attempt to untangle timing. Notably, the meta-analysis of studies predicting children’s externalizing problems revealed that prenatal stress effects on child externalizing did not vary based on type and timing of psychological distress during pregnancy (Tung et al, Reference Tung, Hipwell, Grosse, Battaglia, Cannova, English, Quick, Llamas, Taylor and Foust2023), although effects were larger for earlier outcome timepoints and greater instability of distress between the pre- and postnatal period was associated with larger effects on children’s behavior.

Again, work reviewed above considering stressor exposures across maternal life course has shown independent effects of childhood and pregnancy adversity on child risk for psychopathology (and physical health problems); and maternal childhood trauma exposures have been associated with lasting effects on the functioning of women’s reproductively-relevant biological systems during adulthood (Aschbacher et al., Reference Aschbacher, Hagan, Steine, Rivera, Cole, Baccarella, Epel, Lieberman and Bush2021) and pregnancy specifically (Steine et al., Reference Steine, LeWinn, Lisha, Tylavsky, Smith, Bowman, Sathyanarayana, Karr, Smith, Kobor and Bush2020), moreso than pregnancy stress exposures. Our group even found that maternal histories of childhood trauma amplified the harmful association between pregnancy stressors and children’s mental health (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Rudd, Ahmad, Loftus, Swan, Nguyen, Barrett, Tylavsky, Mason, Karr, Sathyanarayana and LeWinn2023), demonstrating how exposure across sensitive periods might interact, potentiating risk at later time points. As noted earlier, DOHaD science will benefit from more carefully considering exposures to life stressors prior to conception, as limiting study to prenatal exposures obscures etiologic understanding of the impact of risk exposures that occurred earlier in maternal lives. Pinpointing the most impactful windows of development for upstream trauma prevention will be time well spent.

Considering postnatal stress and depression

Related to sensitive periods challenges, it is often challenging to know how and when to adjust for postnatal maternal distress when attempting to untangle prenatal effects on offspring. It is likely that in most contexts, both prenatal programing of fetal development and postnatal difference in childrearing environments (such as through stress-related behavioral changes in parenting) account for associations found. While it is important to adjust for postnatal environment, we must consider that postnatal distress is often a mediator or a factor that lies “on the path” between prenatal stressors and child outcomes, thus default adjustment may eliminate variance of interest, depending upon your question. One way to unpack this involves modeling repeated measures of maternal distress during and after pregnancy and examining the transactional relations with child developmental outcomes. Strong examples include Browne et al. (Reference Browne, LeWinn, May, Tylavsky and Bush2022) and Roubinov et al. (Reference Roubinov, Browne, LeWinn, Lisha, Mason and Bush2022) cross-lagged models that showed pre- and postnatal direct and indirect effects of maternal histories of trauma on children’s developmental and mental health outcomes. Other approaches examine, in sensitivity models, how inclusion of postnatal stress or depression affects the patterns of findings. For example, the effects for both maternal childhood and pregnancy stressors maintained significance, although effects on child mental health were attenuated, after adjustment for maternal postnatal depression (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Rudd, Ahmad, Loftus, Swan, Nguyen, Barrett, Tylavsky, Mason, Karr, Sathyanarayana and LeWinn2023; Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Epel, Vieten, Adler, Laraia, Jones-Mason, Alkon and Bush2022), and adjusting for maternal depression reduced the effect size of maternal maltreatment on offspring’s depression and internalizing disorders (Su et al., Reference Su, D’Arcy and Meng2022). It is also important that models consider including adjustment for current maternal depression and other various maternal factors that may impact her report of child functioning, although this effect has been found to be negligible (Olino et al., Reference Olino, Michelini, Mennies, Kotov and Klein2021). Postnatal distress, including perceived stress, anxiety, and depression must be acknowledged and modeled as one likely mediator of intergenerational transmission of stress in addition to its potential role as a confounder.

A related major critique of much of the work in this domain is that maternal stress exposure during childhood and pregnancy are typically assessed retrospectively and may suffer from recall bias, particularly due to concern that histories of trauma will cloud perceptions of the past. Prospective collection of stress exposure is certainly ideal, however retrospective reports of the occurrence of major negative life events (such as divorce, death of a close relative, incarceration, homelessness, or severe illness) have been found to be valid and robust to recall bias over time (Krinsley et al., Reference Krinsley, Gallagher, Weathers, Kutter and Kaloupek2003; Ramos et al., Reference Ramos, Marceau, Neiderhiser, De Araujo-Greecher, Natsuaki and Leve2020). It is also difficult to capture total pregnancy exposures without waiting until after the birth, given its unpredictable timing and the difficulty accessing birthing parents during the precious period surrounding birth.

Sex-specific effects?

The biological sex of the child in utero, and the associated sex-specific differences in intrauterine hormonal milieu, may play an important role in the impact of maternal stress exposures during pregnancy on child developmental outcomes (Bale & Epperson, Reference Bale and Epperson2015). Numerous theoretical papers and reviews articulate the rationale for sex differences in prenatal programing, and a considerable body of evidence from non-human animal experiments support it, however the broader literature in human studies remains inconclusive. For example, some prior work has found evidence for sex-specific effects of maternal pregnancy distress on young child internalizing (Davis & Pfaff, Reference Davis and Pfaff2014; Letourneau et al., Reference Letourneau, Dewey, Kaplan, Ntanda, Novick, Thomas, Deane, Leung, Pon, Giesbrecht and Team2019). In contrast, studies in larger samples at older child ages have found no moderation by child sex in the prediction of maternal report of child internalizing at age 4 (N = 1948; Bush et al., Reference Bush, Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Rudd, Ahmad, Loftus, Swan, Nguyen, Barrett, Tylavsky, Mason, Karr, Sathyanarayana and LeWinn2023); or age 4–13 internalizing (N = 7,944; O'Donnell et al., Reference O'Donnell, Glover, Barker and O'Connor2014); or age 8 youth self-report of depression or anxiety (N = 1,389; Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Epel, Vieten, Adler, Laraia, Jones-Mason, Alkon and Bush2022). The recent metanalysis of maternal distress effects on externalizing (Tung et al., Reference Tung, Hipwell, Grosse, Battaglia, Cannova, English, Quick, Llamas, Taylor and Foust2023) also found no support for child-sex-specific associations. It is quite possible that biological sex differences may be more important for other child outcomes or for developmental periods, such as adolescence, where pubertal onset and associated hormonal shifts play in important role in the development of mental health (Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2009). To truly understand, examinations across all developmental timepoints, across exposure and outcome types, will be important. It is intriguing to note that in one of the few prenatal programing studies of older adults (Hilberdink et al., Reference Hilberdink, Olff, Roseboom, de Rooij and van Zuiden2023) the consequences of pregnancy adversity on psychological health persisted into late adulthood only for men. Certainly, more work in this space will be informative.

Confusion about when to include race/ethnicity in models

An important area for growth in this field, and psychology more broadly, is to evolve our conceptualizations, modeling, and interpretations around indicators of race and ethnicity. In addition to the aspects of this much-needed work described elsewhere in this article, the “simple” issue of considering covariation and confounding in our analytic models is important to untangle. Race is a sociopolitical construct, rather than a biological variable, and thus can serve as a proxy for the impact of racist practices and structural inequality (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Jordan and Clark2022), although using it as a proxy for unmeasured variables requires considerable thought. We need to appreciate the clear guidance provided by experts, reminding the field to shift habits and rethink training when necessary to ensure we are not including race or ethnicity as a “covariate” to capture unmeasured biological differences, not interpreting covariates if they ARE included to address social confounding, and to be sure to not examine group differences by default, without a clear a priori rationale for why processes might differ between cultural groups and a robust modeling framework to specifically test that question (Cogua et al., Reference Cogua, Ho and Mason2019; Duncan & Montoya-Williams, Reference Duncan and Montoya-Williams2024).

Examples from our work reveal how problematic default inclusion of race as a covariate can be. When examining geospatially derived neighborhood environmental quality in a region where race-based housing segregation has long-divided communities and racist policies have created heinous disparities in resources and opportunity, inclusion of a covariate for race eliminated the predictive value of objectively assessed neighborhood quality (Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Epel, Vieten, Adler, Laraia, Jones-Mason, Alkon and Bush2022) and violent crime (Ahmad et al., Reference Ahmad, Rudd, LeWinn, Mason, Murphy, Juarez, Karr, Sathyanarayana, Tylavsky and Bush2022) on early and middle childhood mental health outcomes, potentially misleading researchers to believe neighborhood does not matter for child mental health. Neighborhood resources and burdens are not distributed equally and result from de facto residential racial segregation via redlining (Heard-Garris et al., Reference Heard-Garris, Boyd, Kan, Perez-Cardona, Heard and Johnson2021), and the potential for community social capital to support mental health has been found to differ based on children’s interpersonal experiences of racial/ethnic discrimination (Okuzono et al., Reference Okuzono, Wilson and Slopen2023), thus, future work to untangle intersections between racism and neighborhood quality are important for improving understanding of neighborhood effects and their relationship to maternal stress exposure effects on children’s mental health.

Reflections on lessons learned

Words matter

Over the years of presenting this work, I’ve been struck by a few encounters that highlighted for me how important it is to put oneself in the audiences’ shoes while designing your studies and presenting your findings on this topic. Earlier in my career, I often was so focused on demonstrating the harmful effects of stress during pregnancy in a world not yet aware/convinced that I sometimes failed to consider what it felt like to hear these hypotheses and findings, particularly if listeners were currently pregnant or already had children and struggled with high levels of stress during pregnancy. Such a concern is not specific to pregnancy effects, but can be particularly pronounced for pregnant persons given the dominant culture of blaming mothers for child outcomes (both within and outside of scientific contexts), the relatively little control pregnant people have over most stressors (particularly macro-level factors, such as racism, disparities in pay by gender, and neighborhood violence), the increased risk due to employment stigma/discrimination, and lack of social policies in the U.S. to protect pregnant people. Winett et al. (Reference Winett, Wulf and Wallack2016) wrote an excellent piece regarding considerations in this space, emphasizing strategies we should all take to avoid “mother-blame” in communication about intergenerational health impacts. Further, ask yourself whether your interpretation of the impact of prenatal stress presents what may be appropriate fetal adaptations as “disordered” or “dysregulated”? This is important, both in terms of avoiding pathologizing adaptive coping strategies but also in terms of potential for stigmatizing children who have the misfortune of being reared in contexts of harm. When engaging in this work, take time to ensure that your work and communication about it does not promote stigma, individual blame for societal shortcomings, or a sense of guilt or doom.

A related caution involves use of the word or theories of “resilience”. Focusing on factors that promote resilience has long been considered critical for the study of developmental psychopathology and for identifying buffers to amplify in order to prevent or intervene to support child development (Luthar & Cicchetti, Reference Luthar and Cicchetti2000; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990). The body of work in this domain has exploded the past decade, influenced by developmental systems theory and considerations of cultural processes, and it has evolved considerably (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2016; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Lucke, Nelson and Stallworthy2021; Ungar, Reference Ungar2021), although conceptualizations within psychology have often failed to focus on or acknowledge the societal systems and structures of power and privilege (e.g, racism, colonialism, patriarchy, and capitalism) that greatly affect individual experiences and capacity to adapt or cope with adversities. Some have also shown that expectations of resilience are typically disproportionately placed upon excluded and oppressed individuals and communities and that indicators of “resilience” are, at times, akin to scar tissue or evidence of an adverse event (Suslovic & Lett, Reference Suslovic and Lett2024). Moreover, although many individuals provide high-quality rearing environments despite prior exposure to adversity, we cannot rely on individuals to offset the risk presented by social adversity. To truly understand the phenomenon of intergenerational transmission, the future of this work must consider the long histories of people, the communities they come from, and work from models that center those marginalized by mainstream society as well as the societal systems of harm (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Jones, Patterson, Simon and Yadeta2023; McLean et al., Reference McLean, Fish, Rogers and Syedin press).

Development and mental health are health

Psychology-focused scientists are often siloed, and our work is often considered outside of the context of “health”. Yet, mental health IS health. Development IS health. Also, understanding the etiology of healthy and atypical brain and behavior development is foundational to other fields, such as pediatrics, and to promoting population health more broadly. We must play an active part in bridging fields and ensuring that our developmental and behavioral outcomes research is prominent within the broader health landscape. This means that you should work to publish your findings in journals within and outside of psychology. Medical and public health journals are often less interested in behavioral/mental relative to medical health outcomes, but this appears to be shifting, particularly in the context of U.S. Surgeon General Murthy’s emphasis on post-pandemic child mental health as a public health crisis that requires prioritization (General, 2021).

In a related vein, although there is huge value to deeply focusing on studying a particular child outcome, I have found that my career (and personal interest in my program of research) has benefitted from evaluating the transdiagnostic impact of stress exposures. While one might study a range of diagnostic outcomes within psychology, broadening your study of outcomes to include physical illnesses as well allows you to expand the impact and potential uptake of your work, because it amplifies the range of stakeholders and advocates interested in your findings. For example, if you have deep expertise in children’s stress biology as it relates to depression, you may find it feasible to also evaluate effects on asthma, which is also linked to stress levels. As noted earlier, our team has found that the same maternal prenatal stress exposure indicator stressful life events during the year prior to birth predicts offspring autonomic nervous system reactivity, obesity risk, and child psychopathology (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Jones-Mason, Coccia, Caron, Alkon, Thomas, Coleman-Phox, Wadhwa, Laraia, Adler and Epel2017, Reference Bush, Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Rudd, Ahmad, Loftus, Swan, Nguyen, Barrett, Tylavsky, Mason, Karr, Sathyanarayana and LeWinn2023; Felder et al., Reference Felder, Epel, Coccia, Cordeiro, Laraia, Adler, Coleman-Phox and Bush2020). This transdiagnostic approach is most sensical if you can study outcomes with shared underlying stress-transmission mechanisms and sufficient incidence (health outcomes are often measured categorically) to be studied within your sample’s size (and budget). It also requires careful partnerships with others who can bring the needed relevant expertise outside of your field.

A primary factor in my success in this transdiagnostic space has been multidisciplinary, team science. Multidisciplinary science has a tremendous capacity to advance health sciences. I had the opportunity to pursue my interest in the study of intergenerational transmission while a Robert Wood Johnson Health and Society Scholar at the UCSF-UC Berkeley joint program, where leaders supported me in journeying through literature from a range disciplines. I learned about European epidemiologic famine studies, tightly-controlled experimental cross-fostering animal placental transmission science, and cardiometabolic disease transmission, and I cobbled all of those literatures together with my child clinical intervention science training to support my first NIH-funding application, which leveraged the experimental design of an existing pregnancy wellness-promotion intervention to answer some key questions in prenatal programing. It is important to note that, although I did achieve the aims of that R01, I struggled a lot initially as I attempted to integrate a lot of literatures, and my multidisciplinary study rarely met the ideal standards of each field. For example, to collect behavioral coding and biological data, I needed a smaller, more intensive visit, necessitating a more limited sample size, which prohibits me from publishing those findings in many of the epidemiological and public health journals my work had been inspired by. Through subsequent projects, I’ve partnered with extensive teams representing pediatrics, epidemiology, obstetrics, biological sciences, social work, public health, and beyond, to conduct and publish large-scale etiologic studies, which have found their homes in journals from a broad range of fields. Lately, I oscillate between those worlds, attempting to fill in gaps from multiple directions, striving to leverage learnings from each context to inform the studies I run in the others. Although such complex career interfacings are not for everyone (and wise mentors will tell you to stay focused initially in your career to ensure you achieve the milestones needed to advance), researchers should look across fields for evidence and inspiration, to best advance the science.

Inclusive, reality-focused science

As is true of most fields of science, including clinical psychology (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Bareket-Shavit, Dollins, Goldie and Mortenson2020; Rodriguez-Seijas et al., Reference Rodriguez-Seijas, McClendon, Wendt, Novacek, Ebalu, Hallion, Hassan, Huson, Spielmans, Folk, Khazem, Neblett, Cunningham, Hampton-Anderson, Steinman, Hamilton and Mekawi2023), the field of prenatal programing research is embedded in the context of oppressive systems and has largely failed to contend with systemic racism, sexism, and classism in our work. As my study of this field has expanded, I have been struck by how rarely I was exposed to writings from scholars of color or disciplines that explicitly address structural factors that serve as a fundamental source of many human stressors. Inspired by others endeavoring to acknowledge and self-reflect upon their own contributions to this problem, I have been working to revise my approaches to the scientific and theoretical scholarship in this space, and the work will be ongoing. Future scholars are encouraged to think more critically about how they might address these challenges pervading our academic and clinical institutions to move the field forward in actively anti-racist, anti-sexist, decolonized, and anti-classist manner (Lett et al., Reference Lett, Adekunle, McMurray, Asabor, Irie, Simon, Hardeman and McLemore2022; Rodriguez-Seijas et al., Reference Rodriguez-Seijas, McClendon, Wendt, Novacek, Ebalu, Hallion, Hassan, Huson, Spielmans, Folk, Khazem, Neblett, Cunningham, Hampton-Anderson, Steinman, Hamilton and Mekawi2023). Particularly when discussing solutions, we need to take care not to offer individual-level interventions as the solution to structural toxins (Suslovic & Lett, Reference Suslovic and Lett2024 ). Moreover, when you build your team, remember to seek partnership from scholars from marginalized backgrounds (and do the necessary personal work beforehand and ongoing to ensure your collaboration with them does not perpetuate historic harms [Lett et al., Reference Lett, Adekunle, McMurray, Asabor, Irie, Simon, Hardeman and McLemore2022]), as they have often been excluded or sidelined despite the excellence and advancements that diverse perspectives bring to teams. In particular, our work needs to better center women of color not only to rectify epistemic injustice and ensure lived experience informs the work, but also because the support advances their success in academia, which is critical (Mangurian et al., Reference Mangurian, Spector and Shim2024). Involving nonprofessional “citizen scientists” from the community you are studying also has the capacity to enhance the value of the work, through engaging the expertise of those with positionality, community knowledge and insights, and lived experience (Lett et al., Reference Lett, Adekunle, McMurray, Asabor, Irie, Simon, Hardeman and McLemore2022). To develop those partnerships, we must spend time with community advisory boards, volunteer in the spaces we hope to learn from, and form genuine connections that we lift up to guide the work.

Publish and emphasize null findings

As is true in other fields, our results aren't always in support of our hypotheses or in line with the prior findings of influential others, and to optimize our knowledge, others need to know. Don't neglect publishing sufficiently-powered non-significant results, or you risk contributing to the replication crisis and biasing effects size estimates in meta-analyses. Although reviewers and editors sometimes find the lack of significant associations to be uninteresting, or they demand a greater burden of evidence for null than significant findings (Franco et al., Reference Franco, Malhotra and Simonovits2014), certain strategies can enhance success (Schuengel, Reference Schuengel2022). Although this article has emphasized cases in which stress was significantly associated with child outcomes, the circumstances in which effects were not found (e.g. no total lifetime or pregnancy stress effects on pCRH; the attenuation of maternal ACEs associations with child mental health between ages 4 to 8 years; the lack of moderation by child sex in many studies) are among the most informative.

Use your voice, advance the science

One of the most important lessons I’ve learned is that few people will care about your questions and ideas more than you do, and I was going to need to advocate and initiate to get the data I needed to test my questions. More often than you may realize, you have the capacity to advocate to get your topics measured within studies, and it often will not happen without your exceptional effort. I’ve found this to be the case in both small and large study contexts. For example, my first prenatal programing R01 study was the result of my idea to add a child follow-up component to a pregnancy intervention not designed to consider 2nd generation impacts. As another example, it may not be too late to add pregnancy stress to a study of children. My colleague, Dr Melanie Thomas, wisely added the Pregnancy Stressful Life Events retrospective recall of events scale (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, D. o. R. H., 2005) to our study 6 months after babies were born, and it has been one of the most important, highly predictive measures I’ve ever used. I subsequently advocated to add it, retrospectively 4-8 years after birth, in 3 other extant child cohorts and have found it to be a powerful predictor of a range of physical and mental health outcomes in those contexts, too, resulting in attention from policy-making leaders. This measure is now being added (albeit later than would have been ideal) to the ECHO consortium, where I hope it contributes to years of discovery related to prenatal exposure to maternal stressors. It’s also critical that scholars with positionality that enables power use their voices to advocate for more inclusive science, for richer study of factors like racism, so the burden for this advocacy does not fall upon those that have been and continue to be marginalized and harmed by the structures we need to study.

Policy implications and future directions

Collectively, this research suggests that the causes of psychopathology and other health and disease outcomes take root much earlier than previously understood or currently communicated. The potential impact of this research on population health and policies that support public well-being is substantial. Given this, I would argue that the most important future direction is building partnerships with policy makers and power-brokers to change the landscape in which exposures to adversity are prevalent to identify and prioritize prevention efforts informed by this research!

Our public health priorities must include the elimination or reduction of exposure to severe stressors, and the provision of stress buffers, to break cycles of adversity, improve women’s health across the lifecourse, and bestow greater health onto future generations. National policies that increase resources for pregnant people and other caregivers that have evidence of benefits to expectant and new families include increases in parental leave supports, enhanced insurance coverage, and perinatal cash transfer programs (Roman et al., Reference Roman, Raffo, Zhu and Meghea2014; Troller-Renfree et al., Reference Troller-Renfree, Costanzo, Duncan, Magnuson, Gennetian, Yoshikawa, Halpern-Meekin, Fox and Noble2022; Van Niel et al., Reference Van Niel, Bhatia, Riano, de Faria, Catapano-Friedman, Ravven, Weissman, Nzodom, Alexander, Budde and Mangurian2020). Providing universal access to populations destigmatizes those services, providing further potential benefits in term of stress reduction. Importantly, policies addressing prenatal stress and child health need to consider the diverse needs of individuals with intersecting social identities to reduce disparities and promote equitable health outcomes. As such, the field requires deeper inquiry into modifiable, structural-level health-promoting factors that influence intergenerational pathways underlying health to inform the development of specific policies.

Engaging with policy stakeholders and makers is certainly more challenging to do at early career stages, but research that is designed with these partnerships in mind, informed by input from stakeholders, is much more likely to result in impact. Sometimes it starts small, such as sharing the evidence to empower local community programs that support pregnant people, so they know their work matters for multiple generations and they are encouraged to continue or have helpful empirical information to help with fundraising for their programs. At times, bigger opportunities for impact might materialize, such as the emerging conversations with health care systems to develop partnerships to test health care reimbursement structures that will support pregnant people experiencing high levels of adversity, stress, or mental health challenges as a means of reducing health impairment across two generations. Broad, public health initiatives to promote maternal and child well-being have even greater potential, thus we need to partner evaluate the effectiveness of public health programs aimed at improving maternal nutrition, mental health, and overall well-being – beginning when they are young, well before conceiving.

Prevention of exposure to adversity is ideal, but as noted earlier, some of the most exciting work is that which demonstrates, using quasi or experimental designs, that investments to reduce stress exposure and/or provide supportive buffers to stress can have beneficial effects on offspring development. In addition to the studies with two-generation benefits noted earlier (Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Coccia, Epel, Vieten, Adler, Laraia, Jones-Mason, Alkon and Bush2022), other studies are underway also investigating the potential for psychosocial interventions during pregnancy to promote health and well-being in children (e.g., Davis et al., Reference Davis, Hankin, Swales and Hoffman2018). Researchers might consider a range of interventions that are aimed at improving prenatal conditions, including but not limited to mitigating maternal distress during pregnancy, and evaluating their effects on offspring risk for psychopathology. There is also value to identifying optimal timing and refinements via cultural adaptations to approaches for interventions that show positive influences child health outcomes. To develop impactful programs and policies that will positively benefit children and their families, more sophisticated examinations of modifiable sources of potential resilience and protection, particularly those at the level of systems, are needed. As many risk-focused studies fail to assess such indicators, researchers need to endeavor to include more of these measures in their studies, using target-community input to ensure designs are culturally-informed and tailored for sharing back useful findings to support the communities. Intervention development should occur in partnership with community stakeholders, centering those most affected, to mitigate the negative effects of adversity across generations. That said, although interventions are promising, it is important to recognize that, despite the increasing efforts to screen individuals for histories of adversity or active distress, the shortage of available effective resources to address harms makes prevention the more sound target.

Beyond those imperatives, some other promising avenues for future investigation include more explicit examination of the impact of social determinants of health (e.g., socioeconomic factors, access to healthcare, neighborhood environmental conditions) during pregnancy on prenatal programing of offspring health disparities. To untangle these structural factors, large datasets are required. Readers should be aware of the publicly available Environmental Child Health Outcomes (ECHO) program database, which offers multidisciplinary opportunities to study infant and child neurodevelopment and health outcomes using de-identified longitudinal data from more than 63,000 participants across the U.S. (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Wakschlag, LeWinn, Hertz-Picciotto, Nozadi, Pieper, Lewis, Biezonski, Blair, Deardorff, Neiderhiser, Leve, Elliott, Duarte, Lugo-Candelas, O’Shea, Avalos, Page and Posner2020; LeWinn et al., Reference LeWinn, Caretta, Davis, Anderson and Oken2022). Researchers can submit a request to the NICHD DASH Data Access Committee to access and use these data (See https://echochildren.org/dash/).

As discussed above, future research should include long-term follow-up studies on cohorts with known prenatal stress exposures to prospectively track the health outcomes of individuals into adolescence and adulthood to identify patterns, associations, and potential late-onset consequences of prenatal programing. In addition, studies need to more deeply consider multisystem adversity and resilience-promoting factors that interact to effect outcomes (Ungar, Reference Ungar2021). The study of fathers is notably absent in this field, despite the clear role of coparents in both the biological transmission and their role in buffering stress pre- or postnatally (Cardenas et al., Reference Cardenas, Morris, Marshall, Aviv, Martínez García, Sellery and Saxbe2022). The role of other close kin or primary care providers in exacerbating or buffering prenatal programing effects is also of interest, although, as noted earlier, focusing on more structural, rather than individual, problems should be the priority (Suslovic & Lett, Reference Suslovic and Lett2024).

Advances in a range of technologies (biologic, informatic, etc.) are enabling a range of exciting new areas of study. For example, the increased ease of collecting and processing biomarkers is allowing for a deeper understanding of processes by which stress transmission can occur and unveils potential targets for interventions (Shonkoff et al., Reference Shonkoff, Boyce, Bush, Gunnar, Hensch, Levitt, Meaney, Nelson, Slopen, Williams and Silveira2022; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Roubinov, Noroña-Zhou and Bush2024). Research into epigenetic mechanisms for the impacts of stress on fetal development is burgeoning (Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020; Yehuda & Lehrner, Reference Yehuda and Lehrner2018), and further exploration of epigenetic modifications, how the epigenetic landscape influences gene expression and phenotype, and the association between epigenetic marks and susceptibility to diseases is likely to be fruitful. Our team recently demonstrated linkages between maternal childhood and pregnancy psychosocial adversity exposures and placental transcriptomic signatures derived from RNA sequencing that have been implicated in adverse pregnancy, birth, and child health outcomes (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Freije, MacDonald, Bammler, Benson, Carroll, Enquobahrie, Karr, LeWinn, Zhao, Bush, Sathyanarayana and Paquette2024), highlighting additional biological pathways of importance. Several research teams are currently investigating the role of maternal pregnancy health conditions, including gestational diabetes, hypertension, and immune activation, as well as the maternal and fetal microbiome (Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019; Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020) in prenatal programing. Placental CRH is often noted a key maternal-fetal stress-transmission factor in reviews (Entringer et al., Reference Entringer, Buss and Wadhwa2015; Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019), although we were surprised to note it was predicted by maternal childhood trauma, but not total lifetime or pregnancy stressors in our large, southern sample (Steine et al., Reference Steine, LeWinn, Lisha, Tylavsky, Smith, Bowman, Sathyanarayana, Karr, Smith, Kobor and Bush2020). Greater understanding of the influence of a vast array of factors outside of social stress on pCRH levels and rise, as well as more accessible rigorous assays of this complex hormone, is likely necessary before the role of this biomarker in stress transmission is clarified. Wearables, particularly those that provide repeated reliable measurements of pregnant people’s arousal and recovery from remote locations while they go about their daily lives, are another avenue that will enhance our ability to monitor and understand the dynamic processes occurring during prenatal development. Delving into these exciting areas in future research may help to unravel the complexities of prenatal programing and pave the way for innovative, holistic prevention programs and interventions that positively influence health across multiple generations.

Conclusion

Mental health is a foundational component of the health of our population. Identifying risk and protective factors for developmental psychopathology, including factors preceding birth, is critical for preventing long-standing impairment to individuals, as well as social and economic costs to society. Collectively, evidence to date points to the need to simultaneously consider stressors experienced across multiple key developmental periods in the mother’s lifespan, and their potential interaction, to capture the intergenerational impact of maternal stressors on her child’s developmental and psychological outcomes. The historical and cultural contexts of these stressors is also critical to consider, as intergenerational transmission isn't limited to two generations. The prevalence of adversity nationally and globally compels us to consider major stressors during childhood and pregnancy need to be important targets for prevention efforts, with potential benefits across generations. That said, many individuals live with histories of harm, and effective interventions during pregnancy to improve outcomes for caregivers and their children are being identified. As scholars, our work advancing this science must ensure that we do not solely frame our questions or solutions at the level of the individual, and instead work to understand the influences of larger context of social and structural determinants in which individuals develop. Moreover, we have the opportunity to bring rigorous methods from the field of developmental psychopathology to partner with other fields, build multidisciplinary teams, and address intersectional challenges to developing optimal health.

Prenatal stress science findings from our field have the capacity to influence policies at the local, state, and national level, as well as within institutions (health care, education, insurance companies, employers). The next generation of research should be designed, in partnership with those in the communities of interest and other key stakeholders, with those targets in mind. The next generation of researchers in this space have an opportunity to use our science in the pursuit of social justice and the potential to play a powerful role combating the current mental health pandemic.

Funding statement

The insights and ideas shared in this paper have been greatly influenced by my collaborators over the years, including colleagues and trainees, but particularly by Dr Amanda Norona-Zhou, who provided helpful comments on this writing. The work owes a debt to my mentors as well, particularly the late Dr Nancy Adler who first supported my research in this area and whose inclusive sponsorship reduced my stress during my own pregnancy, and Dr Elissa Epel who tirelessly helped me obtain and successfully implement my first funded study in this domain – their support, alongside that provided by the wonderful Dr Tom Boyce, created a deeply-appreciated, enriched “gestational environment” for my initial foray into the field. The ideas within have deep “preconception” roots in the work of the illustrious Dr Dante Cicchetti, whose theories and science have long informed my developmental thinking and critical lens of this science, and whose encouragement and high expectations across the years bolstered my ambitions and confidence to test my ideas. Dr Bush receives support from the NIH ECHO Program (UG3OD035519) and funding provided to the Intergenerational Developmental Health Program (IDHP; https://devhealth.ucsf.edu) from the Lisa Stone Pritzker Family Foundation, and she is the Lisa and John Pritzker Distinguished Professor of Developmental and Behavioral Health.

Competing interests

None.

References

Ahmad, S. I., Rudd, K. L., LeWinn, K. Z., Mason, W. A., Murphy, L., Juarez, P. D., Karr, C. J., Sathyanarayana, S., Tylavsky, F. A., & Bush, N. R. (2022). Maternal childhood trauma and prenatal stressors are associated with child behavioral health. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, 13(4), 483493. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2040174421000581 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahmad, S. I., Shih, E. W., LeWinn, K. Z., Rivera, L., Graff, J. C., Mason, W. A., Karr, C. J., Sathyanarayana, S., Tylavsky, F. A., & Bush, N. R. (2022). Intergenerational transmission of effects of women’s stressors during pregnancy: Child psychopathology and the protective role of parenting. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 0. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.838535 Google Scholar
Amani, B., Atkinson, S., Krzeczkowski, J., Mortaji, N., Schmidt, L. A., & Van Lieshout, R. J. (2023). Early neurodevelopment in the offspring of women enrolled in a randomized controlled trial assessing the effectiveness of a nutrition + exercise intervention on the cognitive development of 12-month-olds. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, 14(4), 532539. https://doi.org/10.1017/S204017442300020X Google Scholar
Anderson, R. E., Jones, S. C. T., Patterson, A., Simon, C. B., & Yadeta, K. (2023). When resilience is not enough: Imagining novel approaches to supporting black youth navigating racism. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000986 Google Scholar
Arnold, R., Ahmed, F., Clarke, A., Quinn, N., Beenstock, J., & Holland, P. (2023). The relationship between parental adverse childhood experiences and the health, well-being and development outcomes of their children: A systematic review. Public Health, 219, 146153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2023.03.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aschbacher, K., Hagan, M., Steine, I. M., Rivera, L., Cole, S., Baccarella, A., Epel, E. S., Lieberman, A., & Bush, N. R. (2021). Adversity in early life and pregnancy are immunologically distinct from total life adversity: Macrophage-associated phenotypes in women exposed to interpersonal violence. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 391. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01498-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, B. H., Freije, S., MacDonald, J. W., Bammler, T. K., Benson, C., Carroll, K. N., Enquobahrie, D. A., Karr, C. J., LeWinn, K. Z., Zhao, Q., Bush, N. R., Sathyanarayana, S., & Paquette, A. G. (2024). Placental transcriptomic signatures of prenatal and preconceptional maternal stress. Molecular Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-02403-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, J. R., Caspi, A., Meehan, A. J., Ambler, A., Arseneault, L., Fisher, H. L., Harrington, H., Matthews, T., Odgers, C. L., Poulton, R., Ramrakha, S., Moffitt, T. E., & Danese, A. (2021). Population vs Individual Prediction of Poor Health From Results of Adverse Childhood Experiences Screening. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(4), 385393. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.5602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bale, T. L., & Epperson, C. N. (2015). Sex differences and stress across the lifespan [review article]. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 14131420. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4112 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, D. J. (1990). The fetal and infant origins of adult disease. BMJ, 301(6761), 11111111. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.301.6761.1111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, D. J. (2007). The origins of the developmental origins theory. Journal of Internal Medicine, 261(5), 412417. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2796.2007.01809.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brave Heart, M. Y. H., Chase, J., Elkins, J., & Altschul, D. B. (2011). Historical trauma among indigenous peoples of the americas: Concepts, research, and clinical considerations. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 43(4), 282290. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2011.628913 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne, D. T., LeWinn, K. Z., May, S. S., Tylavsky, F., & Bush, N. R. (2022). Maternal early exposure to violence, psychopathology, and child adaptive functioning: Pre- and postnatal programming. Pediatric Research, 92(1), 9197. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-022-01954-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, B. E., Jordan, A., & Clark, U. S. (2022). Race as a social construct in psychiatry research and practice. JAMA Psychiatry, 79(2), 9394. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.2877 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, E. R., Farr, S. L., & Howards, P. P. (2015). Stressful life events experienced by women in the year before their infants’ births — United States, 2000-2010. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 64(9), 247251.Google ScholarPubMed
Bush, N. R., & Boyce, W. T. (2014). The contributions of early experience to biological development and sensitivity to context. In Lewis, M., & Rudolph, K. D. (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (3rd ed. pp. 287309). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, N. R., & Boyce, W. T. (2016). Differential sensitivity to context: Implications for developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 107137). John Wiley and Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Bush, N. R., Jones-Mason, K., Coccia, M., Caron, Z., Alkon, A., Thomas, M., Coleman-Phox, K., Wadhwa, P. D., Laraia, B. A., Adler, N. E., & Epel, E. S. (2017). Effects of pre- and postnatal maternal stress on infant temperament and autonomic nervous system reactivity and regulation in a diverse, low-income population. Development and Psychopathology, 29(5), 15531571. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001237 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, N. R., Noroña-Zhou, A., Coccia, M., Rudd, K. L., Ahmad, S. I., Loftus, C. T., Swan, S. H., Nguyen, R. H. N., Barrett, E. S., Tylavsky, F. A., Mason, W. A., Karr, C. J., Sathyanarayana, S., & LeWinn, K. Z. (2023). Intergenerational transmission of stress: Multi-domain stressors from maternal childhood and pregnancy predict children’s mental health in a racially and socioeconomically diverse, multi-site cohort. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 58(11), 16251636. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-022-02401-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, N. R., Obradovic, J., Adler, N. E., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Kindergarten stressors and cumulative adrenocortical activation: The “first straws” of allostatic load? Development and Psychopathology, 23(4), 10891106. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579411000514 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, N. R., Wakschlag, L. S., LeWinn, K. Z., Hertz-Picciotto, I., Nozadi, S. S., Pieper, S., Lewis, J., Biezonski, D., Blair, C., Deardorff, J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Leve, L. D., Elliott, A. J., Duarte, C. S., Lugo-Candelas, C., O’Shea, T. M., Avalos, L. A., Page, G. P., & Posner, J. (2020). Family environment, neurodevelopmental risk, and the environmental influences on child health outcomes (ECHO) initiative: Looking back and moving forward. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00547 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardenas, S. I., Morris, A. R., Marshall, N., Aviv, E. C., Martínez García, M., Sellery, P., & Saxbe, D. E. (2022). Fathers matter from the start: The role of expectant fathers in child development. Child Development Perspectives, 16(1), 5459. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12436 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, D. o. R. H (2005). Phase 5 core questionnaire-pregnancy stressful life events. Pregnancy Risk Assessment Monitoring System (PRAMS). http://www.cdc.gov/prams/pdf/phase5_corequestions.pdf.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(04), 597600. https://doi.org/doi:10.1017/S0954579400007318 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cogua, J., Ho, K. Y., & Mason, W. A. (2019). The peril and promise of racial and ethnic subgroup analysis in health disparities research. Journal of Evidence-Based Social Work, 16(3), 311321. https://doi.org/10.1080/26408066.2019.1591317 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conradt, E., Carter, S. E., & Crowell, S. E. (2020). Biological embedding of chronic stress across two generations within marginalized communities. Child Development Perspectives, 14(4), 208214. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12382 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crenshaw, K. (1991). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law Review, 43(6), 12411299. https://doi.org/10.2307/1229039 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dashorst, P., Mooren, T. M., Kleber, R. J., de Jong, P. J., & Huntjens, R. J. C. (2019). Intergenerational consequences of the Holocaust on offspring mental health: A systematic review of associated factors and mechanisms. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1), 1654065. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1654065 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Hankin, B. L., Swales, D. A., & Hoffman, M. C. (2018). An experimental test of the fetal programming hypothesis: Can we reduce child ontogenetic vulnerability to psychopathology by decreasing maternal depression? Development and Psychopathology, 30(3), 787806. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000470 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., & Narayan, A. J. (2020). Pregnancy as a period of risk, adaptation, and resilience for mothers and infants. Development and Psychopathology, 32(5), 16251639. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001121 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., & Pfaff, D. (2014). Sexually dimorphic responses to early adversity: Implications for affective problems and autism spectrum disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 49, 1125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.06.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degruy-Leary, J. (2017). Post-traumatic slave syndrome: America’s legacy of enduring injury. Joy DeGruy Publications Inc.Google Scholar
DiPietro, J. A. (2004). The role of prenatal maternal stress in child development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 7174, http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/13/2/71.full.pdf CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, A. F., & Montoya-Williams, D. (2024). Recommendations for reporting research about racial disparities in medical and scientific journals. JAMA Pediatrics, 178(3), 221–224. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2023.5718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23(01), 728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000611 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., & Del Giudice, M. (2014). Beyond allostatic load: Rethinking the role of stress in regulating human development. Development and Psychopathology, 26(01), 120. https://doi.org/doi:10.1017/S0954579413000849 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Entringer, S., Buss, C., & Wadhwa, P. D. (2015). Prenatal stress, development, health and disease risk: A psychobiological perspective-2015 Curt Richter award paper. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 366375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.019 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans-Campbell, T. (2008). Historical trauma in American Indian/Native alaska communities: A multilevel framework for exploring impacts on individuals, families, and communities. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23(3), 316338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260507312290 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felder, J. N., Epel, E., Coccia, M., Cordeiro, A., Laraia, B., Adler, N., Coleman-Phox, K., & Bush, N. R. (2020). Prenatal maternal objective and subjective stress exposures and rapid infant weight gain. The Journal of Pediatrics, 222, 4551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.03.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, A. J. (1965). Emotional factors in prenatal environment: A review. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 141(1), 108118. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-196507000-00011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franco, A., Malhotra, N., & Simonovits, G. (2014). Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer. Science, 345(6203), 15021505. https://doi.org/doi:10.1126/science.1255484 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenberger, D. J., Clements-Nolle, K., & Yang, W. (2015). The association between adverse childhood experiences and alcohol use during pregnancy in a representative sample of adult women. Women’s Health Issues, 25(6), 688695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.whi.2015.06.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
General, U. S. P. H. S. O. o. t. S. (2021). Protecting youth mental health: The U.S. surgeon general’s advisory. Rockville. https://www.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/surgeon-general-youth-mental-health-advisory.pdf.Google Scholar
Geronimus, A. T. (2013). Deep integration: Letting the epigenome out of the bottle without losing sight of the structural origins of population health. American Journal of Public Health, 103(S1), S56S63. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2013.301380 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geronimus, A. T., Bound, J., & Hughes, L. (2023). Trend toward older maternal age contributed to growing racial inequity in very-low-birthweight infants in the US. Health Affairs, 42(5), 674682. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2022.01066 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glover, V., O'Donnell, K. J., O'Connor, T. G., & Fisher, J. (2018). Prenatal maternal stress, fetal programming, and mechanisms underlying later psychopathology—A global perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 30(3), 843854. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941800038X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluckman, P. D., Low, F. M., Buklijas, T., Hanson, M. A., & Beedle, A. S. (2011). How evolutionary principles improve the understanding of human health and disease. Evolutionary Applications, 4(2), 249263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-4571.2010.00164.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J. D., & Griggs, C. (2009). Developmental changes in hypothalamus-pituitary–adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: Normative changes and associations with puberty. Development and Psychopathology, 21(1), 6985. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankerson, S. H., Moise, N., Wilson, D., Waller, B. Y., Arnold, K. T., Duarte, C., Lugo-Candelas, C., Weissman, M. M., Wainberg, M., Yehuda, R., & Shim, R. (2022). The intergenerational impact of structural racism and cumulative Trauma on depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 179(6), 434440. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.21101000 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heard-Garris, N., Boyd, R., Kan, K., Perez-Cardona, L., Heard, N. J., & Johnson, T. J. (2021). Structuring poverty: How racism shapes child poverty and child and adolescent health. Academic Pediatrics, 21(8), S108S116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2021.05.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heard-Garris, N. J., Cale, M., Camaj, L., Hamati, M. C., & Dominguez, T. P. (2018). Transmitting trauma: A systematic review of vicarious racism and child health. Social Science & Medicine, 199, 230240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.04.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heart, M. Y. H. B. (1999). Oyate ptayela: Rebuilding the lakota nation through addressing historical trauma among lakota parents. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2(1-2), 109126. https://doi.org/10.1300/J137v02n01_08 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertzman, C. (1998). The case for child development as a determinant of health. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 89(Suppl 1), S14-19S16-21, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=9654787,Google ScholarPubMed
Hilberdink, C. E., Olff, M., Roseboom, T. J., de Rooij, S. R., & van Zuiden, M. (2023). The impact of adversities across the lifespan on psychological symptom profiles in late adulthood: A latent profile analysis. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, 14(4), 508522. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2040174423000181 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howland, M. A., Sandman, C. A., & Glynn, L. M. (2017). Developmental origins of the human hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Expert Review of Endocrinology & Metabolism, 12(5), 321339. https://doi.org/10.1080/17446651.2017.1356222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones-Mason, K., Coccia, M., Alkon, A., Melanie Thomas, K. C.-P., Laraia, B., Adler, N., Epel, E. S., & Bush, N. R. (2023). Parental sensitivity modifies the associations between maternal prenatal stress exposure, autonomic nervous system functioning and infant temperament in a diverse, low-income sample. Attachment & Human Development, 25(5), 487523. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2023.2257669 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, K., Murphey, D., Daily, S., Ryberg, R., & Laurore, J. (2020). Maternal and child health inequities emerge even before birth. State of Babies Yearbook.Google Scholar
Kim, A. W., Said Mohamed, R., Norris, S. A., Richter, L. M., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2023). Psychological legacies of intergenerational trauma under South African apartheid: Prenatal stress predicts greater vulnerability to the psychological impacts of future stress exposure during late adolescence and early adulthood in Soweto, South Africa. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(1), 110124. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13672 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krinsley, K. E., Gallagher, J. G., Weathers, F. W., Kutter, C. J., & Kaloupek, D. G. (2003). Consistency of retrospective reporting about exposure to traumatic events. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(4), 399409. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1024474204233 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Letourneau, N., Dewey, D., Kaplan, B. J., Ntanda, H., Novick, J., Thomas, J. C., Deane, A. J., Leung, B., Pon, K., Giesbrecht, G. F., & Team, t A. S. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of adverse childhood experiences via maternal depression and anxiety and moderation by child sex. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, 10(1), 8899. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2040174418000648 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lett, E., Adekunle, D., McMurray, P., Asabor, E. N., Irie, W., Simon, M. A., Hardeman, R., & McLemore, M. R. (2022). Health equity tourism: Ravaging the justice landscape. Journal of Medical Systems, 46(3), 17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10916-022-01803-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeWinn, K. Z., Caretta, E., Davis, A., Anderson, A. L., & Oken, E. (2022). SPR perspectives: Environmental influences on child health outcomes (ECHO) program: Overcoming challenges to generate engaged, multidisciplinary science. Pediatric Research, 92(5), 12621269. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-021-01598-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, X., Laplante, D. P., Elgbeili, G., & King, S. (2023). Preconception and prenatal maternal stress are associated with broad autism phenotype in young adults: Project ice storm. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, 14(4), 481489. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2040174423000156 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lugo-Candelas, C., Talati, A., Glickman, C., Hernandez, M., Scorza, P., Monk, C., Kubo, A., Wei, C., Sourander, A., & Duarte, C. S. (2023). Maternal mental health and offspring brain development. An Umbrella Review of Prenatal Interventions. Biological Psychiatry, 0(0), 934941. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2023.01.026 Google Scholar
Luthar, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Implications for interventions and social policies. Development and Psychopathology, 12(4), 857885. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579400004156 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, X., Biaggi, A., Sacchi, C., Lawrence, A. J., Chen, P. J., Pollard, R., Matter, M., Mackes, N., Hazelgrove, K., Morgan, C., Harding, S., Simonelli, A., Schumann, G., Pariante, C. M., Mehta, M., Montana, G., Rodriguez-Mateos, A., Nosarti, C., & Dazzan, P. (2022). Mediators and moderators in the relationship between maternal childhood adversity and children’s emotional and behavioural development: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 52(10), 18171837. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291722001775 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madigan, S., Deneault, A.-A., Racine, N., Park, J., Thiemann, R., Zhu, J., Dimitropoulos, G., Williamson, T., Fearon, P., Cénat, J. M., McDonald, S., Devereux, C., & Neville, R. D. (2023). Adverse childhood experiences: A meta-analysis of prevalence and moderators among half a million adults in 206 studies. World Psychiatry, 22(3), 463471. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.21122 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangurian, C., Spector, N. D., & Shim, R. S. (2024). Centering women of color to promote excellence in academic medicine. New England Journal of Medicine, 390(2), 102104. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp2309206 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2(4), 425444. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400005812 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2016). Resilience in development: Progress and transformation. In Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention. (vol. 4, 3rd ed. pp. 271333). John Wiley & Sons, Inc, https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy406 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Lucke, C. M., Nelson, K. M., & Stallworthy, I. C. (2021). Resilience in development and psychopathology: Multisystem perspectives. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 521549. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-120307 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLean, K. C., Fish, J., Rogers, L., & Syed, M. (in press). Integrating systems of power and privilege in the study of resilience. American Psychologist, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/yhmj9 Google Scholar
McLemore, M. R., Altman, M. R., Cooper, N., Williams, S., Rand, L., & Franck, L. (2018). Health care experiences of pregnant, birthing and postnatal women of color at risk for preterm birth. Social Science & Medicine, 201, 127135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.02.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mersky, J. P., & Janczewski, C. E. (2018). Racial and ethnic differences in the prevalence of adverse childhood experiences: Findings from a low-income sample of U.S. women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 76, 480487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.12.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monk, C., Lugo-Candelas, C., & Trumpff, C. (2019). Prenatal developmental origins of future psychopathology: Mechanisms and pathways. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15(1), 317344. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moog, N. K., Cummings, P. D., Jackson, K. L., Aschner, J. L., Barrett, E. S., Bastain, T. M., Blackwell, C. K., Bosquet Enlow, M., Breton, C. V., Bush, N. R., Deoni, S. C. L., Duarte, C. S., Ferrara, A., Grant, T. L., Hipwell, A. E., Jones, K., Leve, L. D., Lovinsky-Desir, S., Miller, R. K., Monk, C., Oken, E., Posner, J., Schmidt, R. J., Wright, R. J., Entringer, S., Simhan, H. N., Wadhwa, P. D., O'Connor, T. G., Musci, R. J., & Buss, C. (2023). Intergenerational transmission of the effects of maternal exposure to childhood maltreatment in the USA: A retrospective cohort study. The Lancet Public Health, 8(3), e226e237. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(23)00025-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, T. T., Criss, S., Kim, M., De La Cruz, M. M., Thai, N., Merchant, J. S., Hswen, Y., Allen, A. M., Gee, G. C., & Nguyen, Q. C. (2023). Racism during pregnancy and birthing: Experiences from Asian and Pacific Islander, black, Latina, and middle eastern women. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities, 10(6), 30073017. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40615-022-01475-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noroña-Zhou, A. N., Ashby, B. D., Richardson, G., Ehmer, A., Scott, S. M., Dardar, S., Marshall, L., & Talmi, A. (2023). Rates of preterm birth and low birth weight in an adolescent obstetric clinic: Achieving health equity through trauma-informed care. Health Equity, 7(1), 562569. https://doi.org/10.1089/heq.2023.0075 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noroña-Zhou, A. N., Coccia, M., Epel, E., Vieten, C., Adler, N. E., Laraia, B., Jones-Mason, K., Alkon, A., & Bush, N. R. (2022). The effects of a prenatal mindfulness intervention on infant autonomic and behavioral reactivity and regulation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 84(5), 525535, https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000001066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noroña-Zhou, A. N., Coccia, M., Sullivan, A., O’Connor, T. G., Collett, B. R., Derefinko, K., Renner, L. M., Loftus, C. T., Roubinov, D., Carroll, K. N., Nguyen, R. H. N., Karr, C. J., Sathyanarayana, S., Barrett, E. S., Mason, W. A., LeWinn, K. Z., & Bush, N. R. (2023). A multi-cohort examination of the independent contributions of maternal childhood adversity and pregnancy stressors to the prediction of children’s anxiety and depression. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 51, 497512, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-022-01002-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Donnell, K. J., Glover, V., Barker, E. D., & O'Connor, T. G. (2014). The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 393403. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okuzono, S. S., Wilson, J. Jr, & Slopen, N. (2023). Resilience in development: Neighborhood context, experiences of discrimination, and children’s mental health. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 25512559, https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579423001025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olino, T. M., Michelini, G., Mennies, R. J., Kotov, R., & Klein, D. N. (2021). Does maternal psychopathology bias reports of offspring symptoms? A study using moderated non-linear factor analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(10), 11951201. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13394 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organization, W. H. (2021). Violence against women. World Health Organization. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/violence-against-women Google Scholar
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2011). Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity? Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 2938. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000623 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramos, A. M., Marceau, K., Neiderhiser, J. M., De Araujo-Greecher, M., Natsuaki, M. N., & Leve, L. D. (2020). Maternal consistency in recalling prenatal experiences at 6 Months and 8 Years postnatal. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 41(9), 698705. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000841 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, S. O., Bareket-Shavit, C., Dollins, F. A., Goldie, P. D., & Mortenson, E. (2020). Racial inequality in psychological research: Trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Perspectives On Psychological Science, 15(6), 12951309. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620927709 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez-Seijas, C., McClendon, J., Wendt, D. C., Novacek, D. M., Ebalu, T., Hallion, L. S., Hassan, N. Y., Huson, K., Spielmans, G. I., Folk, J. B., Khazem, L. R., Neblett, E. W., Cunningham, T. J., Hampton-Anderson, J., Steinman, S. A., Hamilton, J. L., & Mekawi, Y. (2023). The next generation of clinical-psychological science: Moving toward anti-racism. Clinical Psychological Science, https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026231156545 Google Scholar
Roman, L., Raffo, J. E., Zhu, Q., & Meghea, C. I. (2014). A statewide medicaid enhanced prenatal care program: Impact on birth outcomes. JAMA Pediatrics, 168(3), 220227. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.4347 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roubinov, D., Browne, D., LeWinn, K. Z., Lisha, N., Mason, W. A., & Bush, N. R. (2022). Intergenerational transmission of maternal childhood adversity and depression on children’s internalizing problems. Journal of Affective Disorders, 308, 205212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.04.030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowell, T., & Neal-Barnett, A. (2022) A systematic review of the effect of parental adverse childhood experiences on parenting and child psychopathology. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma 15, 167180. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40653-021-00400-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schickedanz, A., Halfon, N., Sastry, N., & Chung, P. J. (2018). Parents’ adverse childhood experiences and their children’s behavioral health problems. Pediatrics, 142(2). e20180023. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-0023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuengel, C. (2022). Learning to love the null. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines, 63(3), 249251. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13577 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., Bush, N. R., Gunnar, M. R., Hensch, T. K., Levitt, P., Meaney, M. J., Nelson, C. A., Slopen, N., Williams, D. R., & Silveira, P. P. (2022). Translating the biology of adversity and resilience into new measures for pediatric practice. Pediatrics, 149(6), e2021054493. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2021-054493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, M. V., Gotman, N., & Yonkers, K. A. (2016). Early childhood adversity and pregnancy outcomes. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 20(4), 790798. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-015-1909-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sontag, L. W., & Richards, T. (1938). Studies in fetal behavior: I. Fetal heart rate as a behavioral indicator. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, l(17)), i.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steine, I. M., LeWinn, K. Z., Lisha, N., Tylavsky, F., Smith, R., Bowman, M., Sathyanarayana, S., Karr, C. J., Smith, A. K., Kobor, M., & Bush, N. R. (2020). Maternal exposure to childhood traumatic events, but not multi-domain psychosocial stressors, predict placental corticotrophin releasing hormone across pregnancy. Social Science & Medicine, 266, 113461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113461 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockman, J. K., Hayashi, H., & Campbell, J. C. (2014). Intimate partner violence and its health impact on ethnic minority women. Journal of Women’s Health, 24(1), 6279. https://doi.org/10.1089/jwh.2014.4879 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, Y., D’Arcy, C., & Meng, X. (2022). Intergenerational effect of maternal childhood maltreatment on next generation’s vulnerability to psychopathology: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 23(1), 152162. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838020933870 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, A. D. W., Roubinov, D., Noroña-Zhou, A. N., & Bush, N. R. (2024). Do dyadic interventions impact biomarkers of child health? A state-of-the-science narrative review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 162, 106949. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2023.106949 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suslovic, B., & Lett, E. (2024). Resilience is an adverse event: A critical discussion of resilience theory in health services research and public health. Community Health Equity Research & Policy, 44(3), 339–343. https://doi.org/10.1177/2752535X231159721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas-Argyriou, J. C., Letourneau, N., Dewey, D., Campbell, T. S., Giesbrecht, G. F., & APrON Study Team (2021). The role of HPA-axis function during pregnancy in the intergenerational transmission of maternal adverse childhood experiences to child behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 33(1), 284300. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001767. CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troller-Renfree, S. V., Costanzo, M. A., Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., Gennetian, L. A., Yoshikawa, H., Halpern-Meekin, S., Fox, N. A., & Noble, K. G. (2022). The impact of a poverty reduction intervention on infant brain activity. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 119(5), e2115649119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2115649119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tung, I., Hipwell, A. E., Grosse, P., Battaglia, L., Cannova, E., English, G., Quick, A. D., Llamas, B., Taylor, M., & Foust, J. E. (2023). Prenatal stress and externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review and meta-analysis, no pagination specified-no pagination specified. Psychological Bulletin, 150(2), 107–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000407 Google Scholar
Ungar, M. (2021). Multisystemic resilience: Adaptation and transformation in contexts of change. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Bergh, B. R. H., van den Heuvel, M. I., Lahti, M., Braeken, M., de Rooij, S. R., Entringer, S., Hoyer, D., Roseboom, T., Räikkönen, K., King, S., & Schwab, M. (2020). Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 117, 2664. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Niel, M. S., Bhatia, R., Riano, N. S., de Faria, L., Catapano-Friedman, L., Ravven, S., Weissman, B., Nzodom, C., Alexander, A., Budde, K., & Mangurian, C. (2020). The impact of paid maternity leave on the mental and physical health of mothers and children. A Review of the Literature and Policy Implications. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 28(2), 113126. https://doi.org/10.1097/hrp.0000000000000246 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winett, L. B., Wulf, A. B., & Wallack, L. (2016). Framing strategies to avoid mother-blame in communicating the origins of chronic disease. American Journal of Public Health, 106(8), 13691373. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2016.303239 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R., Bell, A., Bierer, L. M., & Schmeidler, J. (2008). Maternal, not paternal, PTSD is related to increased risk for PTSD in offspring of Holocaust survivors. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42(13), 11041111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yehuda, R., & Lehrner, A. (2018). Intergenerational transmission of trauma effects: Putative role of epigenetic mechanisms. World Psychiatry, 17(3), 243257. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20568 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed