Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-7nlkj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-30T13:04:00.290Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Events, Times, and Mohawk Verbal Inflection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2016

Mark Baker
Affiliation:
Rutgers University
Lisa Demena Travis
Affiliation:
McGill University

Abstract

The Mohawk tense-mood-aspect system has many “irregularities” that seem to require a powerful morphological component distinct from syntax. Nevertheless, it is shown that most of these “irregularities” can be explained in syntactic and semantic terms, once the right lexical entries are given. This result is achieved by making two assumptions of general interest: (1) mood is analyzed as the verbal equivalent of specifity in the NP system, and (2) habitual and stative verbs are shown to have time arguments, whereas ordinary eventive verbs have only event arguments. Given this, morpheme positions, cooccurrence restrictions, and the lack of recursion in the inflectional system can be derived from ordinary principles of composition, head movement, and a simple form of blocking. Only the selection of allomorphs is left to a post-syntactic morphology.

Résumé

Résumé

Le système «temps-mode-aspect» du Mohawk comporte plusieurs «irrégularités» qui semblent devoir relever d’une composante morphologique puissante distincte de la syntaxe. Il est cependant démontré que la plupart de ces «irrégularités» peuvent être expliquées en des termes syntaxiques et sémantiques si les entrées lexicales sont formulées adéquatement. Ce résultat est atteint en se basant sur deux hypothèses d’intérêt général: (1) le mode est analysé comme l’équivalent verbal de la spécificité dans le système nominal et (2) les verbes statifs et habituels ont des arguments temps, alors que les verbes événementiels ont uniquement des arguments événement. Ainsi, les positions des morphèmes, les restrictions de cooccurrence et l’absence de recursion dans le système flexionnel peuvent être dérivés des principes usuels de composition, du mouvement de tête et d’une simple forme de blocage. Seule la sélection des allomorphes relève d’une morphologie post-syntaxique.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, Stephen. 1992. A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, Mark. 1985. The Mirror Principle and morphosyntactic explanation. Linguistic Inquiry 16:373415.Google Scholar
Baker, Mark. 1988. Incorporation: A theory of grammatical fonction changing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Baker, Mark. 1996. The poly synthesis parameter. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, Mark. 1997. Unaccusativity and the adjective/verb distinction: English and Mohawk evidence. In Eastern States Conference on Linguistics, ed. Green, Antony Dubach and Motapanyane, Virginia, 1235. Cornell University.Google Scholar
Baker, Mark, and Stewart, O.T.. 1997. Unaccusativity and the adjective/verb distinction: Edo evidence. In Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society 27, ed. Kusumoto, Kiyomi, 33–47. Graduate Linguistic Student Association, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Baker, Mark, and Travis, Lisa. 1997. Mood as verbal definiteness in a “tenseless” language. Natural Language Semantics 5:213269.Google Scholar
Bittner, Maria, and Hale, Kenneth. 1996. The structural determination of Case and agreement. Linguistic Inquiry 27:168.Google Scholar
Bonvillain, Nancy. 1973. A grammar of Akwesasne Mohawk. Ottawa: National Museum of Man.Google Scholar
Chafe, Wallace. 1967. Seneca morphology and dictionary. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, Wallace. 1970. A semantically based sketch of Onondaga. Baltimore: Waverly Press.Google Scholar
Chafe, Wallace. 1980. Consequential verbs in the Northern Iroquoian languages. In Trends in linguistics s.a.M16 American Indian and Indo-European studies, ed. Klar, K., Langdon, M., and Silver, S., 4349. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Comrie, Bernard. 1976. Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Deering, Nora, and Delisle, Helga. 1976. Mohawk: A teaching grammar. Kahnawake, Quebec: Thunderbird Press.Google Scholar
Dowty, David. 1979. Word meaning and Montague Grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Enç, Murvet. 1986. Towards a referential analysis of temporal expressions. Linguistics and Philosophy 9:405426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enç, Murvet. 1987. Anchoring conditions for tense. Linguistic Inquiry 18:633658.Google Scholar
Foster, Michael. 1985. The language of tense, mood, and aspect in Northern Iroquoian descriptions. International Journal of American Linguistics 51:403405.Google Scholar
Foster, Michael. 1986. Updating the terminology of tense, mood, and aspect in Northern Iroquoian descriptions. International Journal of American Linguistics 52:6572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giorgi, Alessandra, and Pianesi, Fabio. 1991. Toward a syntax of temporal representations. Probus 2:187213.Google Scholar
Glasbey, Sheila. 1993. Distinguishing between events and times: some evidence from the semantics of then . Natural Language Semantics 1:285312.Google Scholar
Halle, Morris, and Marantz, Alec. 1993. Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In The view from Building 20: Essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger, ed. Hale, Kenneth and Keyser, S.J., 111176. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Heim, Irene. 1982. The semantics of definite and indefinite noun phrases. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Higginbotham, James. 1985. On semantics. Linguistic Inquiry 16:547594.Google Scholar
Kamp, Hans, and Reyle, Uwe. 1993. From discourse to logic. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Katz, Graham. To appear. Anti neo-Davidsonianism. In Events as grammatical objects, ed. Pustejovsky, James and Tenny, Carol. CSLI, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Kratzer, Angelika. 1989. Stage level and individual level predicates. Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Landman, Fred. 1992. The progressive. Natural Language Semantics 1:132.Google Scholar
Li, Yafei. 1990. X°-binding and verb incorporation. Linguistic Inquiry 21:399426.Google Scholar
Lounsbury, Floyd. 1953. Oneida verb morphology. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Marácz, László. 1989. Asymmetries in Hungarian. Doctoral dissertation, Groningen.Google Scholar
Marantz, Alec. 1984. On the nature of grammatical relations. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marantz, Alec. 1991. Case and licensing. In The 8th Eastern States Conference on Linguistics, ed. Westphal, Germán, Ao, Benjamin, and Chae, Hee-Rahk, 234253. Ohio State University.Google Scholar
McClure, William. 1994. Syntactic projections of the semantics of aspect. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Michelson, Gunther. 1973. A thousand words of Mohawk. Ottawa: National Museum of Man.Google Scholar
Michelson, Karin. 1975. Mohawk aspect suffixes. Master’s thesis, McGill University.Google Scholar
O’Herin, Brian. 1995. Case and Agreement in Abaza. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Ormston, Jennifer. 1993. Some aspects of Mohawk: The system of verbal inflectional categories. Master’s thesis, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec.Google Scholar
Parsons, Terence. 1990. Events in the semantics of English: A study in subatomic semantics. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Postal, Paul. 1979. Some syntactic rules of Mohawk. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 1986. Null objects in Italian and the theory of pro. Linguistic Inquiry 17:501558.Google Scholar
Scott, Kawennisake Shirley. 1991. Old Kahnawake. Kahnawake, Quebec: Kanie’kehaka Raotitiohkwa Press.Google Scholar
Stowell, Timothy. 1996. The phrase structure of tense. In Phrase structure and the lexicon, ed. Rooryck, Johan and Zaring, Laurie, 277292. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Travis, Lisa. 1984. Parameters and effects of word order variation. Doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Travis, Lisa. 1991. Inner aspect and the structure of VP. Paper read at North East Linguistic Society 22, Newark, Delaware.Google Scholar
Travis, Lisa deMena. 1994. Event phrase and a theory of functional categories. In Proceedings of the 1994 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association, ed. Koskinen, Päivi, 559570. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Uesaka, Miwako. 1996. The ‘te-iru’ construction in Japanese: Interaction between aspect and syntax. Master’s thesis, McGill University.Google Scholar
Williams, Edwin. 1989. The anaphoric nature of θ-roles. Linguistic Inquiry 20:425456.Google Scholar
Williams, Marianne Mithun. 1976a. A grammar of Tuscarora. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Williams, Marianne, ed. 1976b. Kanien’kéha’ Okara’shón:’a (MohawkStories). Albany: New York State Museum.Google Scholar
Zagona, Karen. 1990. Times as temporal argument structure. Ms., University of Washington, Seattle.Google Scholar