Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-24T18:42:12.507Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Aversion-preference patterns in amino acid- or protein-deficient rats: a comparison with previously reported responses to thiamin-deficient diets

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 March 2007

Gilles Fromentin
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de neurobiologie des régulations, CNRS UPR 9054, Collège de France, 11 place Marcellin Berthelot, F75231 Paris cedex 05, France
Dorothy W. Gietzen
Affiliation:
Department of Anatomy, Physiology and Cell Biology, School of Veterinary Medicine and Food Intake Laboratory, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA
Stylianos Nicolaidis
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de neurobiologie des régulations, CNRS UPR 9054, Collège de France, 11 place Marcellin Berthelot, F75231 Paris cedex 05, France
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

The purpose of the present experiments was to extend previous data on the strategy used by adult rats to select feed appropriately when faced with diets devoid of protein or an essential amino acid (EAA), and to compare this strategy with that used when facing vitamin (thiamin) deficiency. Rats fed on either balanced or deficient (EAA or protein) diets were offered a choice between a novel deficient and a familiar (deficient or corrected) diet and their choice was monitored. It was shown that protein- and EAA-deficient rats acquired an aversion for their corresponding familiar devoid diet, which by itself promoted a neophilia for the novel diet. This neophilia was not non-specific because protein-deficient rats facing a choice between a protein-devoid and an EAA-devoid diet did exhibit neophilia but only in the short term (less than 5h), and then switched to a preference for the familiar protein-devoid diet. These results show that, in contrast to the case of vitamin deficiency, the protein- or EAA-devoid diet-induced neophilia can be rapidly reversed if the novel feed happens to be more deleterious than the familiar, inappropriate one. This behaviour suggests the existence of sensitive mechanisms able to reveal within a short time the EAA inadequacy of the ingested feed and to adapt the choice for the most appropriate feeds more promptly than in the face of thiamin deficiency. Thus it appears that balancing EAA is more urgent than balancing thiamin.

Type
General Nutrition
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 1997

References

REFERENCES

Baker, B. J., Booth, D. A., Duggan, J. P. & Gibson, E. L. (1987). Protein appetite demonstrated: learned specificity of protein-cue preference to protein need in adult rats. Nutrition Research 7, 481487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, D. A. (1985). Food-conditioned eating preferences and aversions with interoceptive elements: conditioned appetites and satieties. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443, 2341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, D. A. (1987). How to measure learned control of food or water intake. In Feeding and Drinking, pp. 111149 [Toates, F. M. and Rowland, N. E. editors]. London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Deutsch, J. A., Moore, B. A. & Heinrichs, S. C. (1989). Unlearned specific appetite for protein. Physiology and Behavior 46, 619624.Google Scholar
Gietzen, D. W. (1993). Neural mechanisms in the responses to amino acid deficiency. Journal of Nutrition 123, 610625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gietzen, D. W., Leung, P. M. B., Castonguay, T. W., Hartmann, W. J. & Rogers, Q. R. (1986). Time course of food intake and plasma amino acid concentrations in rats fed amino acid imbalanced or deficient diets. In Interaction of the Chemical Senses with Nutrition, pp. 4156 [Kare, M. R. and Brand, J., editors]. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gietzen, D. W., McArthur, L. H., Thiesen, J. C. & Rogers, Q. R. (1992). Learned preference for the limiting amino acid in rats fed a threonine-deficient diet. Physiology and Behavior 51, 909914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halstead, W. C. & Gallagher, B. (1962). Auto regulation of amino acids in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 55, 107111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harper, A. E., Benevenga, N. J. & Wohlhueter, R. M. (1970). Effects of ingestion of disproportionate amounts of amino acids. Physiological Reviews 80, 428458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, L. J., Hargreaves, F. & Ward, A. (1933). Appetite and choice of diet. The ability of the vitamin B deficient rat to discriminate between diets containing and lacking the vitamin. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 113, 161190.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, S. C., Deutsch, J. A. & Moore, B. E. (1990). Olfactory self-selection of protein containing foods. Physiology and Behavior 47, 409413.Google Scholar
Hrupka, B. J., Gietzen, D. W. & Rogers, Q. R. (1994). Dietary limiting amino acid (LAA) concentrations alter diet selection only in LAA deficient rats. FASEB Journal 8, A545.Google Scholar
Leung, P. M. B. & Rogers, Q. R. (1985). Effect of amino acid imbalance and deficiency on dietary choice patterns of rats. Physiology and Behavior 37, 747758.Google Scholar
Leung, P. M. B., Rogers, Q. R. & Harper, A. E. (1968). Effect of amino acid imbalance on dietary choice. Journal of Nutrition 95, 483492.Google Scholar
Naito-Hoopes, M., Gietzen, D. W., McArthur, L. H. & Rogers, Q. R. (1993). Learned preference and aversion for complete and isoleucine-devoid diet. Physiology and Behavior 53, 485494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potier de Courcy, G., Durand, G., Abraham, J. & Gueguen, L. (1989). Recommendations of the feeding conditions of the laboratory animals (rats and mice). In Science des Aliments pp. 209217. Paris: Centre National d'Etudes et de Recommandations sur la Nutrition et l'Alimentation.Google Scholar
Richter, C. P. (1956). Salt appetite in mammals: its dependence on instinct and metabolism. In L'instinct dans le Comportement des Animaux et de l'Homme, pp. 577629 [Autuori, M. editor]. Paris: Masson et Cie.Google Scholar
Rodgers, W. (1967). Specificity of specific hungers. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 64, 4958.Google Scholar
Rodgers, W. & Rozin, P. (1967). Novel food preferences in thiamine-deficient rats and rats recovered from vitamin deficiency. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 63,421428.Google Scholar
Rogers, Q. R. & Harper, A. E. (1970). Selection of a solution containing histidine by rats feeding on an histidine imbalanced diet. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 72, 6671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, Q. R. & Leung, P. M. B. (1973). The influence of amino acids of the neuroregulation of food intake. Federation Proceedings 32, 17091719.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (1965). Specific hunger for thiamin: recovery from deficiency and thiamin preference. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 59, 98101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. (1967). Specific aversions as a component of specific hungers for thiamine. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 64, 237242.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (1968). Specific aversions and neophobia as a consequence of vitamin deficiency and/or poisoning in half wild and domestic rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 66, 8288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. (1976). Selection of foods by rats, humans, and other animals. In Advances in the Study of Behaviour, vol. 6, pp. 2176 [Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. K., Shaw, E. and Beer, C. editors]. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. & Rodgers, W. (1966). Novel-diet preferences in vitamin-deficient rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 61, 14.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Wells, C. & Mayer, J. (1964). Specific hunger for thiamin: vitamin in water versus in food. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 57, 7884.Google Scholar
Said, A. K., Hegsted, D. M. & Hayes, K. C. (1974). Response of adult rats to deficiencies of different essential amino acids. British Journal of Nutrition 31, 4757.Google Scholar
Sanahuja, J. C. & Harper, A. E. (1962). Effect of amino acid imbalance on food intake and preference. American Journal of Physiology, 202, 165170.Google Scholar
Scott, E. M. & Verney, E. (1947). Self selection of diet: the nature of appetites for B vitamins. Journal of Nutrition 37, 471480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, E. M. & Verney, E. (1948). Self selection of diet: the appetite for thiamin. Journal of Nutrition 37, 8191.Google Scholar
Sidransky, H. & Miloslav Rechcigl, J. R. (1962). Chemical pathology of acute amino acid deficiencies: comparison of morphologic and biochemical changes in young rats fed protein-free or threonine diets. Journal of Nutrition 78, 269277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simson, P. C. and Booth, D. A. (1974 a). The rejection of a diet which has been associated with a single administration of an histidine-free amino acid mixture. British Journal of Nutrition 31, 285297.Google Scholar
Simson, P. C. & Booth, D. A. (1974 b). Dietary aversion established by a deficient load: specificity to the amino acid omitted from a balanced mixture. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 2, 481485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viarouge, C., Caulliez, R. & Nicolaïdis, S. (1991). Effects on metabolic and hormonal parameters of monosodium glutamate (umami taste) ingestion in the rat. Physiology and Behavior 49, 10131018.Google Scholar
Zahorik, D. M. & Maier, S. F. (1974). Preference for tastes paired with recovery from thiamin deficiency in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 87, 10831091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed