Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xm8r8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-26T17:44:20.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“I Readed the Book Last Week.” The Role of Dominant Language, Receptive Vocabulary and Language Structure on Morphosyntactic Awareness in Monolingual and Heritage Language Children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 August 2016

DENISE DAVIDSON*
Affiliation:
Loyola University Chicago
SANDRA B. VANEGAS
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Chicago
ELIZABETH HILVERT
Affiliation:
Loyola University Chicago
IEVA MISIUNAITE
Affiliation:
Loyola University Chicago
*
Address for correspondence: Denise Davidson, Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, 1032 W. Sheridan Road, Chicago, IL 60660. ddavids@luc.edu

Abstract

In this research, 5- and 6-year-old monolingual (English) and heritage language (English–Spanish, English–Urdu) children completed a grammaticality judgment test to assess their awareness of grammatically correct and incorrect morphosyntactic structures in English. Results demonstrated that language group differences were minimized when heritage language children exhibited average receptive vocabulary proficiency for the sample, and when more difficult morphosyntactic structures were assessed. In this middle range, only two group differences were found. Our findings highlight the need to consider factors such as receptive vocabulary when assessing morphosyntactic awareness and language group differences.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We would like to thank the children, parents, and staff at Hermosa Community Organization, St. Sylvester School, St. Peter Catholic School, St. Francis of Rome School, the Islamic Center for Chicago, and the Islamic Foundation School who made this study possible. We are grateful to Ramsha Khan for her assistance in translations, recruitment and data collection. Portions of this research were presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, 2011.

References

Assink, E. M. H., & Sandra, D. (Eds.). (2003). Reading complex words: Cross-language studies. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1986). Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child Development, 57, 498510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 117121. doi:10.1111/cdep.12116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2013). Cognitive effects. In Li, P., & Grosjean, F. (Eds.), The psycholinguistics of bilingualism (pp. 193213). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Miller, B. (1999). The problem of age in second-language acquisition: Influences from language, structure, and task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 127145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackmore, A. M., Pratt, C., & Dewsbury, A. (1995). The use of props in a syntactic awareness task. Journal of Child Language, 22, 405421.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2001). Phonology and language use. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology, 24, 291332.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2007). Fostering morphological processing, vocabulary development, and reading comprehension. In Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 78103). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Carr, D. B. (1979). The development of young children's capacity to judge anomalous sentences. Journal of Child Language, 6, 227241.Google Scholar
Coakes, S. J. (2005). SPSS: Analysis without anguish: Version 12.0 for Windows. Queensland, Australia: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cromdal, J. (1999). Childhood bilingualism and metalinguistic skills: Analysis and control in young Swedish-English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 120. doi:10.1017/S0142716499001010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Da Fontoura, H., & Siegel, L. (1995). Reading, syntactic, and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese-English Canadian children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 139153.Google Scholar
Davidson, D., Raschke, V. R., & Pervez, J. (2010). Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu-English) children. Cognitive Development, 25, 166182. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D., & Tell, D. (2005). Monolingual and bilingual children's use of mutual exclusivity in naming whole objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 2545. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.007 Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd Edition). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Lugo, D. E., Padilla, E. R., & Dunn, L. M. (1986). Test de Vocabulario en Imagenes Peabody. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Foursha-Stevenson, C., & Nicoladis, E. (2011). Early emergence of syntactic awareness and cross-linguistic influence in bilingual children's judgments. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 521534. doi:10.1177/1367006911425818 Google Scholar
Galambos, S. J., & Hakuta, K. (1988). Subject-specific and task-specific characteristics of metalinguistic awareness in bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 141162. doi:10.1017/S0142716400006780 Google Scholar
Galambos, S. J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1990). The effects of learning two languages on levels of metalinguistic awareness. Cognition, 34, 156.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2002). Monolingual and bilingual acquisition: Learning different treatments that-trace phenomena in English and Spanish. In Oller, K. D. & Eliers, R. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 220254). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2005). Minority language survival: Input factors influencing the acquisition of Welsh. In Cohen, J., McAlister, K., Rolstad, K., & MacSwan, J. (Eds.): ISB4: Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism, (pp. 852874). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Gaux, C., & Gombert, J. E. (1999). Implicit and explicit syntactic knowledge and reading in pre-adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 17, 169188. doi:10.1348/026151099165212 Google Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J. (1995). Language differentiation in early bilingual language development. Journal of Child Language, 22, 611631. doi:10.1017/S0305000900009971 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Gombert, J. E. (1997). Metalinguistic development in first language acquisition. In van Lier, L. & Corson, D. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language education, vol. 6: Knowledge about language (pp. 4351). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism. Language and Cognition, 1, 131149.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2010). Bilingual: Life and reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2012). Portraying heritage language speakers. Retrieved from https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/life-bilingual/201203/portraying-heritage-language-speakers Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V., Restrepo, A., & Simón-Cereijido, G. (2006). Evaluating the discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12091223. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/087) Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V., & Simón-Cereijido, G. (2007). The discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Latino English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 968981. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/068) Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 127. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000759 Google Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). Stage/structure vs. phase/process in modeling linguistic and cognitive development. In Levin, I. (Ed.), Stage and structure: Reopening the debate. Norwood, NJ: Albex.Google Scholar
Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2008). Conceptual and methodological issues in comparing metalinguistic awareness across languages. In Koda, K. & Zehler, A.M. (Eds.), Learning to read across languages: Cross-linguistic relationships in first and second language literacy development (pp. 3967). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Lipka, O., Siegel, L. S., & Vukovic, R. (2005). The literacy skills of English language learners in Canada. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 20, 3049. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2005.00119.x Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N. (2010). How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 37, 817840. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990055 Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381401. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.06.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2008). Grammaticality judgments in children: The role of age, working memory and phonological ability. Journal of Child Language, 35, 247268. doi:10.1017/S0305000907008367 Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2009). Second language acquisition in early childhood. Zeitshrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 28, 534. doi:10.1515/ZFSW.2009.002 Google Scholar
Montrul, S. A. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism, Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. A. (2012). Is the heritage language like a second language? In Roberts, L., Lindqvist, C., Bardel, C., & Abrahamsson, N., N. (Eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook 2012 (pp. 129). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. A. (2016). The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Moyle, M. J., Ellis Weismer, S., Lindstrom, M., & Evans, J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late talking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 508528. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/035) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagy, W. E. (2007). Metalinguistic awareness and the vocabulary-comprehension connection. In Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 5277). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching & learning vocabulary. Heinle & Heinle Publishers: Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Nation, I. S. P. (2013). Learning vocabulary in another language (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E. (2006). Cross-linguistic transfer in adjective-noun strings by preschool bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 1532. doi:10.1017/S136672890500235X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2010). Bilingual children's acquisition of English verb morphology: Effects of language exposure, structure complexity, and task type. Language Learning, 60, 651680. doi:10.111/j.1467-9922.2010.00567.x Google Scholar
Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E., Crago, M., & Genesee, F. (2011). Bilingual children's acquisition of the past tense: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 38, 554578. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000218 Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Narrative competence among monolingual and bilingual school children in Miami. In Oller, K. D. & Eilers, R. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 135147). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pratt, C., Tunmer, W. E., & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Children's capacity to correct grammatical violations in sentences. Journal of Child Language, 11, 129141.Google Scholar
Rainey, V. R., Davidson, D., & Li-Grining, C. (2015). Executive functions as predictors of syntactic awareness in English monolingual and English-Spanish bilingual language brokers and nonbrokers. Applied Psycholinguistics. Advance online publication. doi:10.1017/SO142716415000326 Google Scholar
Rothou, K. (2015). The relationship between morphosyntactic awareness and reading comprehension in a transparent language. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Honolulu, Hawaii.Google Scholar
Simard, D., Fortier, V., & Foucambert, D. (2013). Measuring the metasyntactic ability of Portuguese speaking children living in a French speaking environment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 1931. doi:10.1017/S1366728911000071 Google Scholar
Tabachnick, G. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Taft, M. (2003). Morphological representation as a correlation between form and meaning. In Assink, E., & Sandra, D. (Eds.), Reading complex words: Cross-language studies (pp. 113137). New York, NY: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takacs, S. (2007). The strategic use of morphological information in children struggling with reading comprehension (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Simon Frasier University, Canada.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E., Rothenberg, A., Rivard, M. E., & Naves, R. (2006). Bilingual language assessment: Can overall proficiency be estimated from separate measurement of two languages? Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 4, 121.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Valdés, G. (2001). Heritage language students: profiles and possibilities. In Peyton, J., Ranard, J., & McGinnis, S. (Eds.), Heritage languages in America: Preserving a national resource (pp. 3780). McHenry, IL: The Center for Applied Linguistics and Delta Systems.Google Scholar
van Hout, A., Harrigan, K., & de Villiers, J. (2009). Comprehension and production of definite and indefinite noun phrases in English preschoolers. In Crawford, J., Otaki, K., & Takahashi, M. (Eds.): Proceedings of the 3rd conference on generative approaches to language acquisition North America (pp. 7687). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannebaum, K. A. (2007). Promising avenues for better understanding implications of vocabulary development for reading comprehension. In Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 276291). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Yip, V. (2013). Simultaneous language acquisition. In Grosjean, F., & Ping, L. (Eds.), The psycholinguistics of bilingualism (pp. 119136). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2006). Assessing language dominance in bilingual acquisition: A case for mean length utterance differentials. Language Assessment Quarterly, 3, 97116.Google Scholar