Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T01:16:19.241Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How different code-switching types modulate bilinguals’ executive functions: A dual control mode perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2020

Julia Hofweber*
Affiliation:
University College London & University of Reading
Theodoros Marinis
Affiliation:
University of Konstanz & University of Reading
Jeanine Treffers-Daller
Affiliation:
University of Reading
*
Address for correspondence: Julia Hofweber, E-mail: j.hofweber@ucl.ac.uk

Abstract

Most existing studies on the relationship between code-switching and executive functions have focused on experimentally induced language-switching, which differs fundamentally from naturalistic code-switching. This study investigated whether and how bilinguals’ code-switching practices modulate different aspects of executive functioning. Our findings suggest that existing processing models of code-switching should be extended by a dual control mode perspective, differentiating between reactive and proactive monitoring. Bilinguals engaging in code-switching types that keep languages more separate (Alternation) displayed inhibitory advantages in a flanker task inducing reactive control. Dense code-switching, which requires bilinguals to constantly monitor cross-linguistic competition, explained performance in proactive monitoring conditions. Furthermore, a correlation between Dense code-switching and response inhibition suggests that linguistic co-activation may persist during articulatory stages of language processing. Crucially, bilinguals outperformed monolinguals at those aspects of the executive system that were trained by their most frequent code-switching habits. This underlines the importance of sociolinguistic variables in bilingualism research.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abutalebi, J and Green, D (2007) Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of neurolinguistics 20, 242275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antoniou, K, Grohmann, KK, Kambanaros, M and Katsos, N (2016) The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition 149, 1830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Backus, A (2014) Towards a usage-based account of language change: Implications of contact linguistics for linguistic theory. Questioning language contact: Limits of contact, contact at its limits, 91118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badiola, L, Delgado, R, Sande, A and Stefanich, S (2018) Code-switching attitudes and their effects on acceptability judgment tasks. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8, 524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bak, TH (2016) Cooking pasta in La Paz. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6, 699717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, AL, Valdes Kroff, JR and Dussias, PE (2018) From the Field to the Lab: A Converging Methods Approach to the Study of Codeswitching. Languages 3, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhatt, RM and Bolonyai, A (2011) Code-switching and the optimal grammar of bilingual language use. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14, 522546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, HK and Marian, V (2014) Cognitive control in bilinguals: Advantages in Stimulus–Stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17, 610629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 12, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Craik, FI, Grady, C, Chau, W, Ishii, R, Gunji, A and Pantev, C (2005) Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage 24, 4049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, F and Luk, G (2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition 34, 859873.Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, FI and Luk, G (2012) Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in cognitive sciences 16, 240250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K and Griffin, ZM (2000) The Persistence of Structural Priming: Transient Activation or Implicit Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 129, 177192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botero, C, Bullock, B, Davis, K and Toribio, AJ (2004) Perseverative phonetic effects in bilingual code-switching. In 34th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages, University of Utah, March.Google Scholar
Botvinick, MM, Braver, TS, Barch, DM, Carter, CS and Cohen, JD (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological review 108, 624652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braver, TS (2012) The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends in cognitive sciences 16, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, SM and Meltzoff, AN (2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental science 11, 282298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clyne, MG (2003) Dynamics of language contact: English and immigrant languages. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colzato, LS, Bajo, MT, van den Wildenberg, W, Paolieri, D, Nieuwenhuis, S, La Heij, W and Hommel, B (2008) How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34, 302312Google ScholarPubMed
Costa, A, Hernández, M and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 106, 5986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A, Hernández, M, Costa-Faidella, J and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition 113, 135149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bruin, A and Della Sala, S (2016) The importance of language use when studying the neuroanatomical basis of bilingualism. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31, 335339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Groot, AM and Christoffels, IK (2006) Language control in bilinguals: Monolingual tasks and simultaneous interpreting. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9, 189201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Pisapia, N and Braver, TS (2006) A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing 69, 13221326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M, Muysken, P and Wang, SL (2007) Structured variation in codeswitching: towards an empirically based typology of bilingual speech patterns. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 10, 298340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, JM and Wei, L (2014) Attitudes towards code-switching among adult mono-and multilingual language users. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 35, 235251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, JA, Hernández, JA, Antón, E, Macizo, P, Estévez, A, Fuentes, LJ and Carreiras, M (2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality?. Experimental psychology 61, 234251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eppler, EM (2005) The syntax of German–English code-switching (Doctoral dissertation, University of London).Google Scholar
Eppler, ED (2010) “Ich spreche Englisch, ich spreche Deutsch, aber was ist das für eine Sprache?” Emigranto–a grammatical approach to code-switching. AAA-Arbeiten aus Anglistik und Amerikanistik 35, 165187Google Scholar
Field, A (2009) Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage publications.Google Scholar
Fisher, RA (1955) Statistical methods and scientific induction. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society (B) 17, 6978.Google Scholar
Friedman, NP, Miyake, A, Young, SE, DeFries, JC, Corley, RP and Hewitt, JK (2008) Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137, 201225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner-Chloros, P (2009) Code-switching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner-Chloros, P, McEntee-Atalianis, L and Paraskeva, M (2013) Code-switching and pausing: an interdisciplinary study. International Journal of Multilingualism 10, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, LR, January, D, Nappa, R and Trueswell, JC (2007) On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language 57, 544569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goral, M, Campanelli, L and Spiro, A (2015) Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18, 7989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DW (1986) Control, activation, and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and language 27, 210223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DW (1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 1, 6781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DW and Abutalebi, J (2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 515530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DW and Wei, L (2014) A control process model of code-switching. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29, 499511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F (2016) The complementarity principle and its impact on processing, acquisition, and dominance. In Silva-Corvalán, C and Treffers-Daller, J (eds), Language Dominance in Bilinguals: Issues of Measurement and Operationalization. Cambridge: Cambridge Univeristy Press, pp. 6684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F and Miller, JL (1994) Going in and out of languages: An example of bilingual flexibility. Psychological science 5, 201206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guo, T, Liu, H, Misra, M and Kroll, JF (2011) Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. NeuroImage 56, 23002309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartanto, A and Yang, H (2016) Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 150, 1019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofweber, J, Marinis, T and Treffers-Daller, J (2016) Effects of dense code-switching on executive control. Linguistic approaches to bilingualism 6, 648668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofweber, J, Marinis, T & Treffers-Daller, J (2019) Predicting executive functions in bilinguals using ecologically valid measures of code-switching behaviour. In: Sekerina, IA, Spradlin, L, and Valian, V (eds), Bilingualism, Executive Functions, and Beyond. Questions and Insights. Studies in Bilingualism. Amsterdam: Benjamins, pp. 181205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kang, C & Lust, B (2019) Code-switching does not predict Executive Function performance in proficient bilingual children: Bilingualism does. Bilingualism, Language and Cognition 22, 366382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khakimov, N (2014) ‘Muslim-y'and ‘Katholik-en’: Explaining variation in plural marking of German noun insertions in Russian sentences. Language Contact: The State of the Art, 4445.Google Scholar
Kleinman, D and Gollan, TH (2016) Speaking two languages for the price of one: Bypassing language control mechanisms via accessibility-driven switches. Psychological science 27, 700714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, JF, Bobb, SC and Wodniecka, Z (2006) Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9, 119135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, JF, Dussias, PE, Bogulski, CA and Kroff, JRV (2012) Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In Psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press, Vol. 56, pp. 229262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T and Van de Weijer, J (2016) The role of the childhood environment for language dominance: A study of adult simultaneous bilingual speakers of German and French. In: Silva-Corvalan, C and Treffers-Daller, J (eds), Language Dominance in Bilinguals. Issues of Measurement and Operationalization. Cambridge University Press, pp. 174195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehtonen, M, Soveri, A, Laine, A, Järvenpää, J, de Bruin, A and Antfolk, J (2018) Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological bulletin 144, 394425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, WJ, Roelofs, A and Meyer, AS (1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and brain sciences 22, 138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, P, Zhang, F, Tsai, E and Puls, B (2014) Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17(3), 673680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G, Anderson, JA, Craik, FI, Grady, C and Bialystok, E (2010) Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and cognition 74, 347357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luk, G and Bialystok, E (2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 605621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V, Spivey, M and Hirsch, J (2003) Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain and language 86, 7082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Rhee, MM and Bialystok, E (2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: language and cognition 11, 8193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J, Yudes, C, Gómez-Ariza, CJ and Bajo, MT (2015) Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia 66, 157169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muysken, P (2000) Bilingual speech: A typology of code-mixing. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Muysken, P (2013) Language contact outcomes as the result of bilingual optimization strategies. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 16, 709730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers-Scotton, C (2006) Natural codeswitching knocks on the laboratory door. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9, 203212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ooi, S, Goh, W, Sorace, A and Bak, T (2018) From Bilingualism to Bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21, 867879. doi:10.1017/S1366728918000020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, KR and Greenberg, ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive psychology 66, 232258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, Myuz, HA, Anders, RT, Bockelman, MF, Mikulinsky, R and Sawi, OM (2017) No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29, 89112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poarch, GJ and Bialystok, E (2015) Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review 35, 113124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A and Gollan, TH (2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17, 682691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, JC (1998) Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford pyschologists Press.Google Scholar
Rodriguez-Fornells, A, Kramer, U, Lorenzo-Seva, U, Festman, J and Münte, TF (2012) Self-assessment of individual differences in language switching. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruigendijk, E, Hentschel, G and Zeller, JP (2016) How L2-learners’ brains react to code-switches: an ERP study with Russian learners of German. Second Language Research 32, 197223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidlhofer, B (2001) Closing a conceptual gap: The case for a description of English as a lingua franca. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 11, 133158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A, Rodriguez-Fornells, A and Laine, M (2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and EFs in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Struys, E, Surmont, J, Van de Craen, P, Kepinska, O and Van den Noort, M (2019) Bilingual language control across modalities: The relationship between mixed-language comprehension and production. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 127.Google Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J (1992) French-Dutch codeswitching in Brussels: social factors explaining its disappearance, Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 13, 143156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J (1998) The IC model and code-switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, 9899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J (2009) Code-switching and transfer: an exploration of similarities and differences. In: Bullock, BE and Toribio, JA (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Codeswitching. Cambridge University Press, pp. 5874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J, Fernandez, CB, Kootstra, GJ, Litcofsky, KA and Ting, CY (2018) Electrophysiological and experimental-behavioral approaches to the study of intra-sentential code-switching. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8, 134161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valk, PO (2014) Convergent developments in Dutch Turkish word order–A comparative study using ‘elicited production'and ‘judgment'data: Converging evidence?. Applied Linguistics Review 5, 353374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verreyt, N, Woumans, E, Vandelanotte, D, Szmalec, A and Duyck, W (2016) The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19, 181190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997) Wechsler memory scale (WMS-III) (Vol. 14). San Antonio, TX: Psychological corporation.Google Scholar
Weissberger, GH, Wierenga, CE, Bondi, MW and Gollan, TH (2012) Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging 27, 959974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woumans, E and Duyck, W (2015) The bilingual advantage debate: Moving toward different methods for verifying its existence. Cortex 73, 356357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yim, O and Bialystok, E (2012) Degree of conversational code-switching enhances verbal task switching in Cantonese–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15, 873883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar