Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m8s7h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T12:58:57.576Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Factors affecting the retention of sentential negation in heritage Egyptian Arabic*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 June 2014

ABDULKAFI ALBIRINI*
Affiliation:
Utah State University
ELABBAS BENMAMOUN
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
*
Address for correspondence: Abdulkafi Albirini, Department of Languages, Philosophy, and Communication Studies, Utah State University, 0720 Old Main, Logan, UT 84322, USAabdulkafi.albirini@usu.edu

Abstract

This study investigates the areas of resilience and vulnerability in sentential negation in heritage Egyptian Arabic and explores their theoretical implications. Egyptian heritage speakers completed three narrative production tasks, five experimental production tasks, and a acceptability judgment task. The results indicate that they have a full grasp of the location of negation and its configurational properties, but diverge from native speakers in such aspects of sentential negation as merger with lexical heads and dependency or licensing relations. We propose that these asymmetric patterns are due to various factors, including the age at which a structure is typically acquired in the L1, as well as its morphological and syntactic characteristics. The results of this study have implications for the ongoing debate in heritage language research about the linguistic areas that display greater stability/vulnerability. For example, phrase structure seems less vulnerable than licensing dependencies and the mapping between syntax and the morphological interface.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We would like to thank the participants, the research assistants, and Rania Al-Sabbagh for their help with the study. We would also like to thank the three anonymous reviewers and Dr. Robert DeKeyser for their helpful and constructive comments and suggestions. Elabbas Benmamoun's research for this paper was supported in part by the National Science Foundation grant BCS 0826672. Any opinions, findings and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of any agency or entity of the United States Government. We are solely responsible for any errors in the paper.

References

Albirini, A. Toward understanding the variability in the language proficiencies of Arabic heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, doi:10.1177/1367006912472404. Published online by Sage, January 17, 2013.Google Scholar
Albirini, A., & Benmamoun, E. (2014). Aspects of second language transfer in the oral production of Egyptian and Palestinian heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18, 244273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albirini, A., Benmamoun, E., & Chakrani, B. (2013). Gender and number agreement in the oral production of Arabic heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 118.Google Scholar
Albirini, A., Benmamoun, E., & Saadah, E. (2011). Grammatical features of Egyptian and Palestinian Arabic heritage speakers’ oral production. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 273303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alhawary, M. T. (2009). Arabic second language acquisition of morphosyntax. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Aoun, J., Benmamoun, E., & Choueiri, L. (2010). Arabic syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Arab American Institute. (2009). Arab Americans: Demographics. http://www.aaiusa.org/pages/demographics/ (retrieved January 20, 2012).Google Scholar
Bar-Shalom, E., & Zaretsky, E. (2008). Selective attrition in Russian–English bilingual children: Preservation of grammatical aspect. International Journal of Bilingualism, 12, 281302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benmamoun, E. (1992). Inflectional and functional morphology: Problems of projection, representation and derivation. Ph.D. dissertation, University of South California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Benmamoun, E. (2000). The feature structure of functional categories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benmamoun, E., Abunasser, M., Al-Sabbagh, R., Bidaoui, A., & Shalash, D. (2013). The location of sentential negation in Arabic varieties. Brill's Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 5, 84113 Google Scholar
Benmamoun, E., Montrul, S., & Polinsky, M. (2013). Heritage languages and their speakers: Opportunities and challenges for linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics, 39, 129181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broselow, E. (1976). The phonology of Egyptian Arabic. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts at Amherst.Google Scholar
Brustad, K. (2000). The syntax of spoken Arabic. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Choi, H.-W. (2003). Paradigm leveling in American Korean. Language Research, 39, 183204.Google Scholar
Choi, S. (1988). The semantic development of negation: A cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language, 15, 517531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornips, L., & Hulk, A. (2006). External and internal factors in bilingual and bidialectal language development: Grammatical gender of the Dutch definite article. In Lefebvre, C., White, L. & Jourdan, C. (eds.), L2 acquisition and creole genesis: Dialogues, pp. 355378. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cuza, A., & Frank, J. (2011). Transfer effects at the syntax–semantics interface: The case of double-que questions in heritage Spanish. Heritage Language Journal, 8, 6689.Google Scholar
Dimroth, C. (2010). The acquisition of negation. In Horn, L. R. (ed.), The expression of negation, pp. 3973. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisele, J. (1988). The syntax and semantics of tense, aspect, and time reference in Cairene Arabic. Ph.D. dissertation, The University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Farawaneh, S., & Ouali, H. (eds.) (2014). Perspectives on Arabic linguistics XXIV–XXV. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fassi Fehri, A. (1993). Issues in the structure of Arabic clauses and words. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, L. (1999). Not a total loss: The attrition of Japanese negation over three decades. In Hansen, L. (ed.), Second language attrition in Japanese contexts, pp. 142153. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoyt, F. (2010). Negative concord in Levantine Arabic. Ph.D. dissertation, The University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Hummer, P., Wimmer, H., & Antes, G. (1993). On the origins of denial negation. Journal of Child Language, 20, 607618.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie und allgemeine Lautgesetze . English translation 1968, Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton Publishers.Google Scholar
Jelinek, E. (1981). On defining categories: Aux and predicate in Egyptian colloquial Arabic. Ph.D. dissertation, The University of Arizona, Tuscon.Google Scholar
Keijzer, M. (2010). The regression hypothesis as a framework for first language attrition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khalafallah, A. (1969). A descriptive grammar of Saʕidi Egyptian colloquial Arabic. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., Montrul, S., & Yoon, J. (2009). Binding interpretations of anaphors by Korean heritage speakers. Language Acquisition, 16, 335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klima, E. (1964). Negation in English. In Fodor, J. D. & Katz, J. (eds.), The structure of language: Readings in the philosophy of language, pp. 246323. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1966). Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, J. & Wales, R. J. (eds.), Psycholinguistic papers, pp. 183208. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Laleko, O. (2010). The syntax–pragmatics interface in language loss: Covert restructuring of aspect in heritage Russian. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Lange, S., & Larsson, K. (1973). Syntactic development of a Swedish girl Embla, between 20 and 42 months of age, part 1: Age 20–25 mo. Stockholm: Institutionen för nordiska språk.Google Scholar
Lynch, A. (2003). The relationship between second and heritage language acquisition: Notes on research and theory building. Heritage Language Journal, 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Major, R. C. (1992). Losing English as a first language. Modern Language Journal, 76, 190208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Books for Young Readers.Google Scholar
McCarthy, C. (2008). Morphological variability in the comprehension of agreement: An argument for representation over computation. Second Language Research, 24, 459486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morpho-syntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 125142.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2011). Interfaces and incomplete acquisition. Lingua, 212, 591604.Google Scholar
Omar, M. (1973). The acquisition of Egyptian Arabic as a native language. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Ouali, H., & Soltan, U. (2014). On negative concord in Egyptian and Moroccan Arabic. In Farawaneh & Ouali (eds.), pp. 159–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ouhalla, J. (1994). Verb movement and word order in Arabic. In Lightfoot, D. & Hornstein, N. (eds.), Verb movement, pp. 4172. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (1997). Cross-linguistic parallels in first language loss. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 14, 87123.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2009). What breaks in A- and A-bar chains under incomplete acquisition. Presented at CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, University of California at Davis.Google Scholar
Pollock, J.-Y. (1989). Verb movement, Universal Grammar, and the structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 365424.Google Scholar
Sherkina-Lieber, M., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., & Johns, A. (2011). Grammar without speech production: The case of Labrador Inuttitut heritage receptive bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 301317.Google Scholar
Shlonsky, U. (1997). Clause structure and word order in Hebrew and Arabic: An essay in comparative Semitic syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1994). Language contact and change: Spanish in Los Angeles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (2003). Linguistic consequences of reduced input in bilingual first language acquisition. In Montrul, S. & Ordóñez, F. (eds.), Linguistic theory and language development in Hispanic languages, pp. 375397. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Smadi, M. (1979). The acquisition of the Jordanian Arabic interrogation and negation by a three-year-old native speaker of Jordanian Arabic. Ph.D. dissertation, The University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Soltan, U. (2007). On formal feature licensing in minimalism: Aspects of Standard Arabic morphosyntax. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park.Google Scholar
Soltan, U. (2011). On issues of Arabic syntax: An essay in syntactic argumentation. Brill's Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 3, 236280.Google Scholar
Soltan, U. (2012). Morphosyntactic effects of NPI-licensing in Cairene Egyptian Arabic: The puzzle of -š disappearance resolved. In Choi, J., Hague, E., Punske, J., Schertz, D. & Trueman, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 29th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, pp. 241249. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Soltan, U. (2014). On the distribution and licensing of polarity-sensitive items in Egyptian Arabic: The cases of ʔayy and walaa. In S. Farawaneh & H. Ouali (eds.), pp. 181–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2000). Differential effects of attrition in the L1 syntax of near-native L2 speakers. In Howell, C., Fish, S. & Keith-Lucas, T. (eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 719725. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Suh, E. (2008). The usage and interpretation of Korean -tul “plural” by heritage language speakers. In Bowles, M., Foote, R., Perpiñán, S. & Bhatt, R. (eds.), Proceedings of the Second Language Research Forum 2007, pp. 239251. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Sorace, A. (2006). Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax–semantics and syntax–discourse phenomena. In Bamman, D., Magnitskaia, T. & Zaller, C. (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 653664. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Watson, J. (2002). The phonology and morphology of Arabic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wode, H. (1977). Four early stages in the development of L1 negation. Journal of Child Language, 4, 87102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, H. (2013). Negation in incomplete language acquisition of Mandarin Chinese. Presented at the 21st Annual Meeting of International Association of Chinese Linguistics, Taipei, Taiwan.Google Scholar