Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-cjp7w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-19T08:32:02.712Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Competition between form-related words in bilingual sentence reading: Effects of language proficiency

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 October 2022

Maud Pélissier*
Affiliation:
Université Paris Cité, CLILLAC-ARP, F-75013 Paris, France
Dag Haugland
Affiliation:
University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
Bjørn Handeland
Affiliation:
University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
Beatrice Zitong Urland
Affiliation:
University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
Allison Wetterlin
Affiliation:
University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
Linda Wheeldon
Affiliation:
University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
Steven Frisson
Affiliation:
University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
*
Address for correspondence: Maud Pélissier, E-mail: maud.pelissier@u-paris.fr

Abstract

Sentence reading involves constant competition between lexical candidates. Previous research with monolinguals has shown that the neighbours of a read word are inhibited, making their retrieval as a subsequent target more difficult, but the duration of this interference may depend on reading skills. In this study, we examined neighbour priming effects in sentence reading among proficient Norwegian–English bilinguals reading in their L2. We investigated the effects of the distance between prime and target (short vs. long) and the nature of the overlap between the two words (beginning or end), and related these to differences in individual cognitive skills. Our results replicated the inhibition effects found in monolinguals, albeit slightly delayed. Interference between form-related words was affected by the L2 reading skills and, crucially, by the phonological decoding abilities of the bilingual reader. We discuss the results in light of competition models of bilingual reading as well as episodic memory accounts.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andrews, S and Hersch, J. (2010) Lexical precision in skilled readers: Individual differences in masked neighbor priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 139, 299318. doi: 10.1037/a0018366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, S and Lo, S. (2012) Not all skilled readers have cracked the code: Individual differences in masked form priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 38, 152163. doi: 10.1037/a0024953Google ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M. (2016) Working memory and language learning: A review. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 0265659016654206. doi: 10.1177/0265659016654206Google Scholar
Baayen, RH, Piepenbrock, R and Gulikers, L. (1996) The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM).Google Scholar
Barr, DJ, Levy, R, Scheepers, C and Tily, H. J. (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 44, 255278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton, K. (2020) MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R package version 1.43.17 (1.43.17). https://cran.r-project.org/package=MuMInGoogle Scholar
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B, Walker, S, Christensen, RHB, Singmann, H, Dai, B, Grothendieck, G and Green, P. (2015) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using ‘Eigen’ and S4. R package, version 1.1-9. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Bijeljac-Babic, R, Biardeau, A and Grainger, J. (1997) Masked orthographic priming in bilingual word recognition. Memory and Cognition 25, 447457. doi: 10.3758/BF03201121Google ScholarPubMed
Borella, E, Carretti, B and Pelegrina, S. (2010) The Specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning Disabilities 43, 541552. doi: 10.1177/0022219410371676CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, JS, Davis, CJ and Hanley, D. A. (2005) Interfering neighbours: The impact of novel word learning on the identification of visually similar words. Cognition 97, B45B54. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridwell, K. (2017) Processing differences in reading alliteration and rhyme: An eye-movement study [University of South Carolina]. https://scholarcommons.sc.edu/senior_theses/137Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M, Lagrou, E and Stevens, M. (2017) Visual word recognition in a second language: A test of the lexical entrenchment hypothesis with lexical decision times. Bilingualism 20, 530548. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M and Stevens, M. (2018) Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of Cognition 1, 120. doi: 10.5334/joc.10Google ScholarPubMed
Chace, KH, Rayner, K and Well, A. D. (2005) Eye movements and phonological parafoveal preview: Effects of reading skill. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 59, 209217. doi: 10.1037/h0087476CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coltheart, M, Davelaar, E, Jonasson, JT and Besner, D. (1977) Access to the internal lexicon. In Dornick, S (ed), Attention and performance VI: Vol. VI. Erlbaum, pp. 535556.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M, Rastle, K, Perry, C, Langdon, R and Ziegler, J. (2001) DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108, 204256. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cop, U, Keuleers, E, Drieghe, D and Duyck, W. (2015) Frequency effects in monolingual and bilingual natural reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 22, 12161234. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0819-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A, Caramazza, A and Sebastian-Galles, N. (2000) The Cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 26, 12831296. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1283Google ScholarPubMed
Daneman, M and Merikle, P. M. (1996) Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 3, 422433. doi: 10.3758/BF03214546CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C. J. (2005) N-watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics. Behavior Research Methods 37, 6570. doi: 10.3758/BF03206399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C. J. (2010) The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review 117, 713758. doi: 10.1037/a0019738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, CJ and Lupker, S. J. (2006) Masked inhibitory priming in English: Evidence for lexical inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32, 668687. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.668Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, CJ and Taft, M. (2005) More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12, 904910. doi: 10.3758/BF03196784CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijkstra, T, Miwa, K, Brummelhuis, B, Sappelli, M and Baayen, H. (2010) How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 62, 284301. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T and van Heuven, WB (1998) The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. In Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. M. (Eds.), Localist Connectionist Approaches To Human Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. doi: 10.4324/9780203774533Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T and van Heuven, WJB (2002) The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5, 175197. doi: 10.1017/s1366728902003012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T and Van Heuven, WJB (2010) Repetition and masked form priming within and between languages using word and nonword neighbors *. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13, 341357. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990575CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S (2008) Phonological priming in auditory word recognition: When both controlled and automatic processes are responsible for the effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 62, 3341. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.62.1.33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, LM, Dunn, LM and Whetton, C (1982) The British Picture Vocabulary Scale. NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Duyck, W, Van Assche, E, Drieghe, D and Hartsuiker, RJ (2007) Visual word recognition by bilinguals in a sentence context: Evidence for nonselective lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 33, 663679. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.663Google Scholar
Duyck, W, Vanderelst, D, Desmet, T and Hartsuiker, R. J. (2008) The frequency effect in second-language visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 15, 850855. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.4.850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsherif, MM, Wheeldon, LR and Frisson, S. (2022) Phonological precision for word recognition in skilled readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 75, 10211040. doi: 10.1177/17470218211046350CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engbert, R, Nuthmann, A, Richter, EM and Kliegl, R. (2005) Swift: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review 112, 777813. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisson, S, Bélanger, NN and Rayner, K. (2014a) Phonological and orthographic overlap effects in fast and masked priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 67, 17421767. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.869614CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frisson, S, Koole, H, Hughes, L, Olson, A and Wheeldon, L. (2014b) Competition between orthographically and phonologically similar words during sentence reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 73, 148173. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, TH, Montoya, RI, Fennema-Notestine, C and Morris, S. K. (2005) Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory and Cognition 33, 12201234. doi: 10.3758/BF03193224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, TH, Slattery, TJ, Goldenberg, D, Van Assche, E, Duyck, W and Rayner, K (2011) Frequency Drives Lexical Access in Reading but Not in Speaking: The Frequency-Lag Hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 140, 186209. doi: 10.1037/a0022256CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, J. (1990) Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Memory and Language 29, 228244. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90074-ACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, 6781. doi: 10.1017/s1366728998000133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, M and Sawyer, M. (1992) L2 Working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 14, 2538. doi: 10.1017/S0272263100010457CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johann, V, Könen, T and Karbach, J. (2020) The unique contribution of working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and intelligence to reading comprehension and reading speed. Child Neuropsychology 26, 324344. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2019.1649381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolliffe, I. (2002) Principal Component Analysis. Springer-Verlag. doi: 10.1007/b98835Google Scholar
Kumle, L, , MLH and Draschkow, D. (2021) Estimating power in (generalized) linear mixed models: An open introduction and tutorial in R. Behavior Research Methods 53, 25282543. doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01546-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A, Brockhoff, PB and Christensen, R. H. B. (2015) lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Package R, Version 2.0-33 (2.0-33). https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Lemhöfer, K, Dijkstra, T, Schriefers, H, Baayen, RH, Grainger, J and Zwitserlood, P. (2008) Native language influences on word recognition in a second language: A megastudy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 34, 1231. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.12Google Scholar
Libben, MR and Titone, DA (2009) Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35(2), 381390. doi: 10.1037/a0014875Google ScholarPubMed
Lukatela, G and Turvey, M. T. (1994) Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 2. Evidence from phonological priming by homophones and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 123, 331353. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luque, A and Morgan-Short, K. (2021) The relationship between cognitive control and second language proficiency. Journal of Neurolinguistics 57, 100956. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100956CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V, Blumenfeld, HK and Kaushanskaya, M. (2007) The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 940967. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, JL and Rumelhart, D. E. (1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88, 375407. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375Google Scholar
Pagán, A, Paterson, KB, Blythe, HI and Liversedge, S. P. (2016) An inhibitory influence of transposed-letter neighbors on eye movements during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 23, 278284. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0869-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, KB, Liversedge, SP and Davis, C. J. (2009) Inhibitory neighbor priming effects in eye movements during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 16, 4350. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfetti, C. (2007) Reading Ability: Lexical Quality to Comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading 11, 357383. doi: 10.1080/10888430701530730Google Scholar
Perfetti, C and Bell, L. (1991) Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming. Journal of Memory and Language 30, 473485. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90017-ECrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, JR, Lupker, SJ and Davis, C. J. (2008) An evaluation of the interactive-activation model using masked partial-word priming. Language and Cognitive Processes 23, 3668. doi: 10.1080/01690960701578112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pivneva, I, Mercier, J and Titone, D. (2014) Executive control modulates cross-language lexical activation during L2 reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 40, 787796. doi: 10.1037/a0035583Google ScholarPubMed
Pollatsek, A, Perea, M and Binder, K. S. (1999) The effects of “neighborhood size” in reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25, 11421158. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.1142Google ScholarPubMed
Powell, M. (2009) The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives. In NA Report NA2009/06. http://www6.cityu.edu.hk/rcms/publications/preprint26.pdfGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K, Pollatsek, A, Ashby, J and Clifton, C. J. (2012) The Psychology of Reading (2nd ed.). Psychology Press. doi: https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203155158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2020) R: A Language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.r-project.org/Google Scholar
Reilly, RG and Radach, R. (2006) Some empirical tests of an interactive activation model of eye movement control in reading. Cognitive Systems Research 7, 3455. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.07.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W (2019) psych: Procedures for personality and psychological research (1.9.12). https://cran.r-project.org/package=psychGoogle Scholar
Rogers, CL, Lister, JJ, Febo, DM, Besing, JM and Abrams, H. B. (2006) Effects of bilingualism, noise, and reverberation on speech perception by listeners with normal hearing. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 465485. doi: 10.1017/s014271640606036xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schröter, P and Schroeder, S. (2018) Differences in visual word recognition between L1 and L2 speakers: The impact of length, frequency, and orthographic neighborhood size in German children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 40, 319339. doi: 10.1017/S0272263117000201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segui, J and Grainger, J. (1990) Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16, 6576. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.65Google ScholarPubMed
Snell, J, van Leipsig, S, Grainger, J and Meeter, M (2018) OB1-reader: A model of word recognition and eye movements in text reading. Psychological Review 125, 969984. doi: 10.1037/rev0000119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, MJ, Stothard, S, Clarke, P, Bowyer-Crane, C, Harrington, A, Truelove, E, Nation, K and Hulme, C. (2009) YARC York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension. GL Publishers. https://www.worldcat.org/title/york-assessment-of-reading-for-comprehension-complete-set/oclc/873011477Google Scholar
Tabachnick, BG and Fidell, L. S. (2007) Using multivariate statistics. (5th Editio). Pearson Allyn and Bacon. https://lccn.loc.gov/2017040173Google Scholar
Tenpenny, P. L. (1995) Abstractionist versus episodic theories of repetition priming and word identification. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2, 339363. doi: 10.3758/BF03210972CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unsworth, SJ and Pexman, P. M. (2003) The impact of reader skill on phonological processing in visual word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology 56 A, 6381. doi: 10.1080/02724980244000206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, JG and Dijkstra, T (2002) Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9, 780789. doi: 10.3758/BF03196335Google ScholarPubMed
Van Heuven, WJB, Dijkstra, T and Grainger, J (1998) Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 39, 458483. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veivo, O and Järvikivi, J. (2013) Proficiency modulates early orthographic and phonological processing in L2 spoken word recognition. Bilingualism 16, 864883. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000600CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, J, Tian, J, Han, W, Liversedge, SP and Paterson, K. B. (2014) Inhibitory stroke neighbour priming in character recognition and reading in Chinese. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 67, 21492171. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.909507CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westfall, J, Kenny, DA and Judd, C. M. (2014) Statistical power and optimal design in experiments in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143, 20202045. doi: 10.1037/xge0000014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiederholt, J and Blalock, G. (2000) GSRT: Gray Silent Reading Tests. PROED Inc. https://www.proedinc.com/Products/9190/gsrt-gray-silent-reading-tests.aspx?bCategory=lra!readGoogle Scholar
Yates, M and Slattery, T. J. (2019) Individual differences in spelling ability influence phonological processing during visual word recognition. Cognition 187, 139149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.02.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: PDF

Pélissier et al. supplementary material

Pélissier et al. supplementary material

Download Pélissier et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 3 MB