Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-hgkh8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:49:05.732Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The cognitive status of metalinguistic knowledge in speakers of one or more languages

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2020

Michael Sharwood Smith*
Affiliation:
Heriot-Watt University
*
Address for correspondence: Michael Sharwood Smith, E-mail: msharwood@blueyonder.co.uk

Abstract

The term ‘metalinguistic’ is used to define the kind of ability whereby people for various purposes view language as an object. It is strongly associated with consciousness and touches on many aspects of literacy, multilingualism and language acquisition. Discussions in the research literature have generally been on specific aspects of metalinguistic knowledge: the time is ripe for a more fundamental reassessment focusing on how exactly metalinguistic ability is represented and processed on line, and how it fits in with other kinds of representation and processing. To this end, a particular theoretical perspective that takes into account contemporary research in cognitive science, the Modular Cognition Framework, will be applied with the aim of supporting further empirical investigations into this area of language ability and locating it within an integrated approach to cognition in general. Finally, the usefulness of metalinguistic knowledge will be briefly considered.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbot-Smith, K, & Tomasello, M (2006) Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition. The Linguistic Review 23, 275290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J, & Lebiere, (eds) (1998) The atomic components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Baars, B (2002) The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 4752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baetens, K, Ma, N, & Van Overwalle, F (2017) The dorsal medial prefrontal cortex is recruited by high construal of non-social stimuli. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 11, 112. doi.org/10.1177/0093854808316275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bardovi-Harlig, K (1999) Exploring the interlanguage of interlanguage pragmatics: A research agenda for acquisitional pragmatics. Language Learning 49, 677713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, HC, & Kurzban, R (2006) Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review 113, 628647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berwick, R, Frederici, A, Chomsky, N, & Bolhuis, J (2013) Evolution, brain and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Science 17, 8998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (1986) Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child development 57, 498510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E (2001) Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annual review of applied linguistics 21, 169181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, & Ryan, EB (1985) Towards a definition of metalinguistic skill. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 31, 229251.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P (2006) The Architecture of the Mind: massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P (2015) Perceiving mental states. Consciousness and Cognition 36, 496507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N (1972) Language and mind (enlarged ed.). New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L, & Tooby, J (1994) Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In Hirschfeld, LA, & Gelman, SA (eds), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 85116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culicover, P, Jackendoff, R, & Audring, J (2017) Multiword Constructions in the Grammar. Topics in Cognitive Science 9, 552568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeKeyser, RM (1995) Learning second language grammar rules. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 17, 379410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R (2003) Implicit and explicit learning. In Doughty, C & Long, M (eds), Handbook of second language acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 313348.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, RM (1997) Beyond explicit rule learning: Automatizing second language morphosyntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 19, 195221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dulay, H, & Burt, M (1974) Natural sequences in child second language acquisition. Language Learning 24, 3753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dulay, H, Burt, M, & Krashen, S (1982) Language Two. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, NC (2011) Implicit and explicit SLA and their interface. In Sanz, C & Leow, R (eds), Implicit and explicit language learning: Conditions, processes, and knowledge in SLA and Bilingualism. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 3547.Google Scholar
Ellis, R (2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 27, 141172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, JA (1983) The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, JM (2015) The prefrontal cortex 5th ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R (1987) Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R (1996) Conceptual semantics and cognitive linguistics. Cognitive Linguistics 7, 93129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R (2002) Foundations of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R (2012) A User's Guide to Thought and Meaning. Oxford: Oxford University PressAQ2.Google Scholar
Jessner, U (2005) Multilingual metalanguage, or the way multilinguals talk about their languages. Language Awareness 14, 5669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A, Grant, J, Sims, K, Jones, M-C., & Cuckl, P (1996) Rethinking metalinguistic awareness: representing and accessing knowledge about what counts as a word. Cognition 58, 197219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiefer, M, & Pulvermüller, F (2010) Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex 48, 805825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S (1978) The Monitor Model of second language performance. In Underhill, R (ed), California Linguistics Association: Proceedings. Campanile Press, pp. 115122.Google Scholar
Krashen, SD (1985) The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Kroll, JF, Bobb, SC, & Hoshino, N (2014) Two languages in mind: Bilingualism as a tool to investigate language, cognition, and the brain. Current directions in psychological science 23, 159163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G (1987) Women, fire and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambon, RM, Jefferies, E, Patterson, K, & Rogers, T (2017) The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. National Reviews of Neuroscience 18, 4255.Google Scholar
Mandler, GA (1975) Consciousness: Respectable, useful and probably necessary. In Solso, R, (ed), Information processing and cognition: the Loyola symposium. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morsella, E, Godwin, C, Jantz, T, Krieger, S, & Gazzaley, A (2016) Passive frame theory: A new synthesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39, 4970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A, Flykt, A, & Lunquist, D (2000) Unconscious emotion: evolutionary perspectives, psychophysiological data and neuropsychological mechanisms. In Lane, RD & Nadel, L (eds), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 296327.Google Scholar
Paradis, M (2014) A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Peters, AM (1983) The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Plonsky, L, Marsden, E, Crowther, D, Gass, S, & Spinner, P (2019) A methodological synthesis and meta-analysis of judgment tasks in second language research. Second Language Research. doi.org/10.1177/0267658319828413. Published online by Sage Journals on March 7, 2019.Google Scholar
Prinz, JJ (2007) All consciousness is perceptual. In McLaughlin, BP, Cohen, J (eds), Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Mind. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing Ltd, pp. 335357.Google Scholar
Rose, K, & Kasper, G (eds), (2001) Pragmatics in Language Teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryle, G (1949) The concept of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R (1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics 11, 1746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, BD (1986) The epistemological status of second language acquisition. Second Language Research 2, 120159.Google Scholar
Sharwood Smith, MA (1996) Metalinguistic ability and primary language data. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19, 740741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M (2004) In two minds about grammar: On the interaction of linguistic and metalinguistic knowledge in performance. Transactions of the Philological Society 102, 255280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Sharwood, M. (2015) In search of conceptual frameworks for relating brain activity to language function. Frontiers in Psychology. 5, 132134.Google Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M (2017) Introducing Language and Cognition: A Map of the Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M (2019) Representing representation: Three perspectives and the case of grammatical gender. In Dominguez, L & Slabakova, L (eds), Proceedings of the 14th generative approaches to second language acquisition conference. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, pp. 241.Google Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M, & Truscott, J (2014a) The multilingual mind: a modular processing perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M, & Truscott, J (2014b) Explaining input enhancement. International Review of Applied Linguistics 52, 253281.Google Scholar
Soon, C, Brass, M, Heinze, H, & Haynes, J.-D. (2008) Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience 11, 543545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorace, A (2011) Pinning Down the Concept of ‘Interface’ in Bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J, & Cosmides, L (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, JH, Cosmides, L and Tooby, J (eds), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Truscott, J (2004) The effectiveness of grammar instruction. Analysis of a metanalysis. English Teaching and Learning 28, 1729.Google Scholar
Truscott, J (2015) Consciousness and second language learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Truscott, J, & Sharwood Smith, M (2004) Acquisition by processing: a modular perspective on language development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truscott, J, & Sharwood Smith, M (2011) Input, intake and consciousness: the quest for a theoretical foundation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33, 297528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truscott, J, & Sharwood Smith, M (2017) Representation, Processing and Code-switching. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition 20, 903916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truscott, J, & Sharwood Smith, M (2019) The internal context of bilingual processing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wray, A (2002) Formulaic language and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar