Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-7qhmt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T06:01:23.123Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sensorimotor grounding and reused cognitive domains

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 October 2010

Maria Brincker
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, Graduate Center, City University of New York, New York, NY 10016. mbrincker@gc.cuny.edusites.google.com/site/mariabrincker/

Abstract

Anderson suggests that theories of sensorimotor grounding are too narrow to account for his findings of widespread “reuse” supporting multiple different cognitive “task domains.” I call some of the methodological assumptions underlying this conclusion into question, and suggest that his examples reaffirm rather than undermine the special status of sensorimotor processes in cognitive evolution.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brincker, M. (forthcoming) Moving beyond mirroring – A social affordance model of sensorimotor integration during action perception. Doctoral dissertation, Department of Philosophy, Graduate Center, City University of New York. (forthcoming in September 2010).Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R. & Schacter, D.L. (2008) The brain's default network: Anatomy, function and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124:138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (1997) Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallese, V. (2003) A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: From control to representation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, B: Biological Sciences 358(1435):1231–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A. (2010) Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 1(4):586–96.Google ScholarPubMed
Graziano, M. S. A., Taylor, C. S. R., Moore, T. & Cooke, D. F. (2002b) The cortical control of movement revisited. Neuron 36:349–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haggard, P., Rossetti, Y. & Kawato, M., eds. (2008) Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G. & Prinz, W. (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(5):849–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurley, S. L. (1998) Consciousness in action. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nunez, R. & Freeman, W. (2000) Reclaiming cognition: The primacy of action, intention, and emotion. Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G. & Matelli, M. (1998) Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Experimental Brain Research 71:491507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar