Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T21:19:00.049Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Extending and refining the fearful ape hypothesis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Tobias Grossmann*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904, USA grossmann@virginia.edu

Abstract

The fearful ape hypothesis (FAH) presents an evolutionary-developmental framework stipulating that in the context of cooperative caregiving, unique to human great ape group life, heightened fearfulness was adaptive. This is because from early in human ontogeny fearfulness expressed and perceived enhanced care-based responding and cooperation with mothers and others. This response extends and refines the FAH by incorporating the commentaries' suggestions and additional lines of empirical work, providing a more comprehensive and nuanced version of the FAH. Specifically, it encourages and hopes to inspire cross-species and cross-cultural, longitudinal work elucidating evolutionary and developmental functions of fear in context. Beyond fear, it can be seen as a call for an evolutionary-developmental approach to affective science.

Type
Author's Response
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidov, M., Paz, Y., Roth-Hanania, R., Uzefovsky, F., Orlitsky, T., Mankuta, D., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2021). Caring babies: Concern for others in distress during infancy. Developmental Science, 24(2), e13016. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidov, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Roth-Hanania, R., & Knafo, A. (2013). Concern for others in the first year of life: Theory, evidence, and avenues for research. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 126131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, T. (2018). How to build a helpful baby: A look at the roots of prosociality in infancy. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 2124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, T., Missana, M., & Krol, K. M. (2018). The neurodevelopmental precursors of altruistic behavior in infancy. PLoS Biology, 16, e2005281. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2005281CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, T., Missana, M., & Vaish, A. (2020). Helping, fast and slow: Exploring intuitive cooperation in early ontogeny. Cognition, 196, 104144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurr, E., Namdari, R., Lai, J., Parker, D., Wendt, D. C., & Burack, J. A. (2020). Perspective on shyness as adaptive from Indigenous peoples of North America. In Schmidt, L. A. & Poole, K. L. (Eds.), Adaptive shyness: Multiple perspectives on behavior and development (pp. 239249). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38877-5_13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155186. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., Muller, K., & Tomasello, M. (2019). The relation between young children's physiological arousal and their motivation to help others. Neuropsychologia, 126, 113119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children are intrinsically motivated to see others helped. Psychological Science, 23, 967972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2017). The fulfillment of others’ needs elevates children's body posture. Developmental Psychology, 53, 100113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. Developmental Science, 14(6), 13931405. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01082.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmboe, K., Bonneville-Roussy, A., Csibra, G., & Johnson, M. H. (2018). Longitudinal development of attention and inhibitory control during the first year of life. Developmental Science, 21, e12690. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12690CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, L., Bugnyar, T., Griesser, M., Hengl, M., Izawa, E. I., Oortwijn, T., … Massen, J. J. (2020). Sex-specific effects of cooperative breeding and colonial nesting on prosociality in corvids. eLife, 9, e58139. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.58139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, M., Muris, P., Loxton, H., & Wege, A. (2017). Anxiety-proneness, anxiety symptoms, and the role of parental overprotection in young South African children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(1), 262270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016-0545-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, N. C., & Roche, M. J. (2018). Current evolutionary adaptiveness of anxiety: Extreme phenotypes of anxiety predict increased fertility across multiple generations. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 106, 8290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2014). Unconscious discrimination of social cues from eye whites in infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 1620816213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Neural signatures of conscious and unconscious emotional face processing in human infants. Cortex, 64, 260270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2020). The developmental origins of subliminal face processing. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 116, 454460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kataja, E. L., Eskola, E., Pelto, J., Korja, R., Paija, S. P., Nolvi, S., … Leppänen, J. M. (2022). The stability of early developing attentional bias for faces and fear from 8 to 30 and 60 months in the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study. Developmental Psychology, 58(12), 22642274. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001432CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Gross, J. N., Lin, M. H., & Nichols, K. E. (2002). Guilt in young children: Development, determinants, and relations with a broader system of standards. Child Development, 73(2), 461482. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00418CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, K. L. (2019). How there got to be so many of us: The evolutionary story of population growth and a life history of cooperation. Journal of Anthropological Research, 75(4), 472497. https://doi.org/10.1086/705943CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kret, M. E., Jaasma, L., Bionda, T., & Wijnen, J. G. (2016). Bonobos (Pan paniscus) show an attentional bias toward conspecifics' emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(14), 37613766. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1522060113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kret, M. E., Muramatsu, A., & Matsuzawa, T. (2018). Emotion processing across and within species: A comparison between humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132(4), 395409. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A. (2015). Neural, cognitive, and evolutionary foundations of human altruism. WIREs Cognitive Science, 7, 5971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R., Lambert, M. L., Frohnwieser, A., Brecht, K. F., Bugnyar, T., Crampton, I., … Clayton, N. S. (2022). Socioecological correlates of neophobia in corvids. Current Biology, 32(1), 7485.e74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.10.045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Missana, M., Altvater-Mackensen, N., & Grossmann, T. (2017). Neural correlates of infants' sensitivity to vocal expressions of peers. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 3944. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.04.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Missana, M., Atkinson, A. P., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Tuning the developing brain to emotional body expressions. Developmental Science, 18, 243253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Missana, M., Rajhans, P., Atkinson, A. P., & Grossmann, T. (2014). Discrimination of fearful and happy body postures in 8-month-old infants: An event-related potential study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 531. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00531CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poole, K. L., & Schmidt, L. A. (2020). Adaptive shyness: A developmental perspective. In Schmidt, L. A. & Poole, K. L. (Eds.), Adaptive shyness: Multiple perspectives on behavior and development (pp. 2540). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38877-5_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajhans, P., Jessen, S., Missana, M., & Grossmann, T. (2016). Putting the face into context: Body expressions impact facial emotion processing in human infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 115121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G. (2016). Cooperation, fast and slow: Meta-analytic evidence for a theory of social heuristics and self-interested deliberation. Psychological Science, 27, 11921206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2013). Human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 413425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth-Hanania, R., Davidov, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2011). Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: Early signs of concern for others. Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 447458. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638311000506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thrasher, C., Krol, K. M., & Grossmann, T. (2021). Mother's engagement with infant linked to infant's responding to threat. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(Suppl 1), e22224. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22224CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231255. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veit, W., & Browning, H. (2022). Pathological complexity and the evolution of sex differences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45, e149. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X22000498CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vreeke, L. J., Muris, P., Mayer, B., Huijding, J., & Rapee, R. M. (2013). Skittish, shielded, and scared: Relations among behavioral inhibition, overprotective parenting, and anxiety in native and non-native Dutch preschool children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(7), 703710. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.09.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yiu, W. Y. V., Choi, J. H., & Chen, X. (2020). Shyness and adaptation across cultures. In Schmidt, L. A. & Poole, K. L. (Eds.), Adaptive shyness: Multiple perspectives on behavior and development (pp. 201218). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38877-5_11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., & Moll, J. (2020). Moral motivation and the basal forebrain. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 108, 207217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaki, J., & Mitchell, J. P. (2013). Intuitive prosociality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 466470. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413492764CrossRefGoogle Scholar