Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-tsvsl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-27T18:21:26.504Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are we virtuously caring or just anxious?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Charlie Kurth*
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI 49008-5328, USA charles.kurth@wmich.edu; www.charliekurth.com

Abstract

According to Grossmann, the high levels of cooperation seen in humans are the result of a “virtuous caring cycle” on which the increased care that more fearful children receive brings increased cooperative tendencies in those children. But this proposal overlooks an equally well supported alternative on which children's anxiety – not a virtuous caring cycle – explains the cooperative tendencies of humans.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barrett, L. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 2858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, O. J. (2011). Maternal nurturing is dependent on her innate anxiety. Hormones and Behavior, 59, 202212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M., Walker, D. L., Miles, L., & Grillon, C. (2010). Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: Role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 105135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, T., & Jessen, S. (2017). When in infancy does the “fear bias” develop? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 153, 149154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hookway, C. (1998). Doubt: Affective states and the regulation of inquiry. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 24(suppl.), 203225.Google Scholar
Insel, T. R. (1990). Regional changes in brain oxytocin receptors post-partum. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 2, 539545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitcher, P. (2011). The ethical project. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krol, K. M., Monakhov, M., Lai, P. S., Ebstein, R., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Genetic variation in CD38 and breastfeeding experience interact to impact infants’ attention to social eye cues. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 112, E5434E5442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurth, C. (2015). Moral anxiety and moral agency. In Timmons, M. (Ed.), Oxford studies in normative ethics (Vol. 5, pp. 171195). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurth, C. (2016). Anxiety, normative uncertainty, and social regulation. Biology & Philosophy, 31, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurth, C. (2018). The anxious mind. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lago, T., Davis, A., Grillon, C., & Ernst, M. (2017). Striatum on the anxiety map: Small detours into adolescence. Brain Research, 1654, 177184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A. (2015). Neural, cognitive, and evolutionary foundations of human altruism. WIREs Cognitive Science, 7, 5971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A. (2008). Fear and anxiety. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 127156). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2013). Life in interesting times. In Sterelny, K., Joyce, R., Calcott, B., & Fraser, B. (Eds.), Cooperation and its evolution (pp. 89108). MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuulari, J. J., Kataja, E. L., Leppänen, J. M., Lewis, J. D., Nolvi, S., Häikiö, T., … Karlsson, H. (2020). Newborn left amygdala volume associates with attention disengagement from fearful faces at eight months. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, 100839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed