Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-5wvtr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T06:24:07.387Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sue Ned Block!: Making a better case for P-consciousness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 March 2008

Victor A. F. Lamme
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands. The Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, part of the Royal Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), 1105 BA Amsterdam, TheNetherlandsv.a.f.lamme@uva.nlwww.cognitiveneuroscience.nl

Abstract

Block makes a case for the existence of conscious experience without access. His case would have been much stronger, however, if he had woven fully unconscious processing into the “mesh argument,” and considered arguments that are intrinsic to neuroscience.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baars, B. J. (2005) Global Workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in Brain Research 150:4553.Google Scholar
Block, N. (2005) Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(2):4652.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.-P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J. & Sergant, C. (2006) Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomoy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(5):204–11.Google Scholar
Haynes, J. D., Driver, J. & Rees, G. (2005) Visibility reflects dynamic changes of effective connectivity between V1 and fusiform cortex. Neuron 46(5):811–21.Google Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F. (2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(1):1218.Google Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F. (2004) Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention; a case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks 17:861–72.Google Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F. & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000) The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neuroscience 23(11):571–79.Google Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F., Zipser, K. & Spekreijse, H. (1998) Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex is suppressed by anesthesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95(6):3263–68.Google Scholar
Landman, R., Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2003) Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research 43(2):149–64.Google Scholar
Lau, H. C. & Passingham, R. E. (2007) Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 27:5805–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholte, H. S., Witteveen, S. C., Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2006) The influence of inattention on the neural correlates of scene segmentation. Brain Research 1076:106–15.Google Scholar
Sergent, C., Baillet, S. & Dehaene, S. (2005) Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nature Neuroscience 8(10):1391–400.Google Scholar
Silvanto, J., Lavie, N. & Walsh, V. (2005b) Double dissociation of V1 and V5/MT activity in visual awareness. Cerebral Cortex 15:1736–41.Google Scholar
Singer, W. (1995) Development and plasticity of cortical processing architectures. Science 270:758–64.Google Scholar
Supèr, H., Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2001b) Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). Nature Neuroscience 4(3):304–10.Google Scholar
Thompson, K. G. & Schall, J. D. (1999) The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking. Nature Neuroscience 2:283–88.Google Scholar