Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T19:15:24.868Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Testing the deficient processing account of the spacing effect in second language vocabulary learning: Evidence from eye tracking

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2019

Natalie G. Koval*
Affiliation:
Michigan State University
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: kovalnat@msu.edu

Abstract

The spacing effect refers to the learning benefit that comes from separating repeated study of target items by time or by other items. A prominent proposed explanation for this effect states that repeated exposures that occur closely together may not engage full attentional processing due to residual activation of the previous exposure and also, in an intentional learning context, due to a sense of familiarity that may result in strategic allocation of less study time to an item in massed repetitions. The present study used eye-tracking methodology to investigate the effects of temporal distribution of repeated exposures to novel second language words on attentional processing and learning of these words under intentional learning instructions. Adult native speakers of English read Finnish words embedded in English sentence contexts under massed and spaced conditions. The results showed that (a) massed repeated exposures received less attentional processing than spaced repeated exposures; (b) target words were better remembered in the spaced condition; and (c) attention was a significant mediator of the obtained spacing effect, in line with the predictions of the deficient processing account of the spacing effect. Implications for vocabulary learning are discussed.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, L. E., Bahrick, A. S., & Bahrick, P. E. (1993). Maintenance of foreign language vocabulary and the spacing effect. Psychological Science, 4, 316321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begg, I., & Green, C. (1988). Repetition and trace interaction: Superadditivity. Memory & Cognition, 16, 232242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellezza, F. S., Winkler, H. B., & Andrasik, F. (1975). Encoding processes and the spacing effect. Memory & Cognition, 3, 451457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamin, A. S., & Tullis, J. G. (2010). What makes distributed practice effective? Cognitive Psychology, 61, 228247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, S. (2010). Effects of distributed practice on the acquisition of second language English syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 635650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjork, R. A., & Allen, T.W. (1970). The spacing effect: Consolidation or differential encoding? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 567572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. C., & Shuell, T. J. (1981). Effects of massed and distributed practice on the learning and retention of second-language vocabulary. Journal of Educational Research, 74, 245248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1972). Stimulus-sampling theory of encoding variability. In Melton, A. W., and Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 85123). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of Cognition, 1, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bui, D. C., Maddox, G. B., & Balota, D. A. (2013). The roles of working memory and intervening task difficulty in determining the benefits of repetition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 341347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callan, D., & Schweighofer, N. (2010). Neural correlates of the spacing effect in explicit verbal semantic encoding support the deficient-processing theory. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 645659.Google ScholarPubMed
Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cepeda, N. J., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., & Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing effects in learning: A temporal ridgeline of optimal retention. Psychological Science, 19, 10951102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Challis, B. H. (1993). Spacing effects on cued-memory tests depend on level of processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 389396.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuddy, L. J., & Jacoby, L. L. (1982). When forgetting helps memory: An analysis of repetition effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 451467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. (2008–). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA): 520 million words, 1990–present. Available online at https://corpus.byu.edu/coca/Google Scholar
Delaney, P. F., Verkoeijen, P., & Spirgel, A. (2010). Spacing and testing effects: A deeply critical, lengthy, and at times discursive review of the literature. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 63147). San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dellarosa, D., & Bourne, L. E. (1985). Surface form and the spacing effect. Memory & Cognition, 13, 529537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dempster, F. N. (1988). The spacing effect: A case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research. American Psychologist, 43, 627634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect: Now you see it, now you don’t. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 795805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das gedächtnis: Untersuchungen zur experimentellen psychologie. Berlin: Duncker & Humblot.Google Scholar
Elgort, I., Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., & Van Assche, E. (2017). Contextual word learning during reading in a second language: An eye-movement study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Elgort, I., & Warren, P. (2014). L2 Vocabulary learning from reading: Explicit and tacit lexical knowledge and the role of learner and item variables. Language Learning, 64, 365414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1955). Statistical theory of distributional phenomena in learning. Psychological Review, 62, 369377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S. (1988). Integrating research areas: A framework for second language studies. Applied Linguistics, 9, 198217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerbier, E., & Toppino, T. C. (2015). The effect of distributed practice: Neuroscience, cognition, and education. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 4, 4959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. (1976). Monotonic and nonmonotonic lag effects in paired-associate and recognition memory paradigms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. (1979). Component-levels theory of the effects of spacing of repetitions on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 7, 95112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A. M., & Smith, S. M. (1981). Spacing repetitions and solving problems are not the same. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 110119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Ahn, J., Choi, I., Ballard, L., Cui, Y., Johnston, S., . . . Yoon, H-J. (2017). Incidental vocabulary learning in a natural reading context: An eye-tracking study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Godfroid, A., Boers, F., & Housen, A. (2013). An eye for words: Gauging the role of attention in incidental L2 vocabulary acquisition by means of eye tracking. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 483517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., & Schmidtke, J. (2013). What do eye movements tell us about awareness? A triangulation of eye-movement data, verbal reports and vocabulary learning scores. In Bergsleithner, J. M., Frota, S. N., and Yoshioka, J. K. (Eds.), Noticing and second language acquisition: Studies in honor of Richard Schmidt (pp. 183205). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai'i, National Foreign Language Resource Center.Google Scholar
Greene, R. L. (1989). Spacing effects in memory: Evidence for a two-process account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 371377.Google Scholar
Greeno, J. G. (1967). Paired-associate learning with short-term retention: Mathematical analysis and data regarding identification of parameters. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 4, 430472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2006). A primer on multilevel modeling. Human Communication Research, 32, 385410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Helsdingen, A., van Gog, T., & van Merriënboer, J. (2011). The effects of practice schedule and critical thinking prompts on learning and transfer of a complex judgment task. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 383398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintzman, D. L., & Block, R. A. (1973). Memory for the spacing of repetitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99, 70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintzman, D. L., Block, R. A., & Summers, J. J. (1973). Modality tags and memory for repetitions: Locus of the spacing effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 12, 229238.Google Scholar
Horst, M., Cobb, T., & Meara, P. (1998). Beyond A Clockwork Orange: Acquiring second language vocabulary through reading. Reading in a Foreign Language, 11, 207223.Google Scholar
Hyönä, J., & Niemi, P. (1990). Eye movements in repeated movements of a text. Acta Psychologica, 73, 259280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: Author.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L. (1978). On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 649667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, W. A., & Uhl, C. N. (1976). The contributions of encoding effort and variability to the spacing effect on free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 153160.Google Scholar
Joseph, H., Wonnacott, E., Forbes, P., & Nation, K. (2014). Becoming a written word: Eye- movements reveal order of acquisition effects following incidental exposure to new words during silent reading. Cognition, 133, 238248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87, 329354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, S., Lindsey, R. V., Mozer, M. C., & Pashler, H. (2014). Retrieval practice over the long term: Should spacing be expanding or equal-interval? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 15441550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapler, I. V., Weston, T., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Spacing in a simulated undergraduate classroom: Long-term benefits for factual and higher-level learning. Learning and Instruction, 36, 3845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, M., Kim, J., & Kwon, J. S. (2001). The effect of immediate and delayed word repetition on event-related potential in a continuous recognition task. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 387396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Learning concepts and categories: Is spacing the “enemy of induction”? Psychological Science, 19, 585592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krug, D., Davis, B., & Glover, J. A. (1990). Massed versus distributed repeated reading: A case of forgetting helping recall? Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 366371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K. (1969). Reinforcement as consolidation. Psychological Review, 76, 8296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laufer, B., & Hulstijn, J. H. (2001). Incidental vocabulary acquisition in a second language: The construct of task-induced involvement. Applied Linguistics, 22, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maddox, G. B. (2016). Understanding the underlying mechanism of the spacing effect in verbal learning: A case for encoding variability and study-phase retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28, 684706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maddox, G. B., & Balota, D. A. (2015). Retrieval practice and spacing effects in young and older adults: An examination of the benefits of desirable difficulty. Memory & Cognition, 43, 760774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madigan, S. A. (1969). Intraserial repetition and coding processes in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 828835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magliero, A. (1983). Pupil dilations following pairs of identical and related to-be-remembered words. Memory & Cognition, 11, 609615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mammarella, N., Avons, S. E., & Russo, R. (2004). A short-term perceptual priming account of spacing effects in explicit cued-memory tasks for unfamiliar stimuli. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 387402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melton, A. W. (1967). Repetition and retrieval from memory. Science, 158, 532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melton, A. W. (1970). The situation with respect to the spacing of repetitions and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 596606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mohamed, A. A. (2017). Exposure frequency in L2 reading: An eye-movement perspective of incidental vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Nakata, T. (2015). Effects of expanding and equal spacing on second language vocabulary learning: Does gradually increasing spacing increase vocabulary learning? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 677711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakata, T., & Webb, S. (2016). Does studying vocabulary in smaller sets increase learning? The effects of part and whole learning on second language vocabulary acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 523552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. New York: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Pashler, H., Zarow, G., & Triplett, B. (2003). Is temporal spacing of tests helpful even when it inflates error rates? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 10511057.Google ScholarPubMed
Pavlik, P. I., & Anderson, J. R. (2005). Practice and forgetting effect on vocabulary memory: An activation-based model of the spacing effect. Cognitive Science, 29, 559586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellicer-Sánchez, A. (2016). Incidental L2 vocabulary acquisition from and while reading: An eye-tracking study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 97130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). Spacing and repetition effects in human memory: Application of the SAM model. Cognitive Science, 27, 431452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 14571506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition, 14, 191201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1987). Eye movements in reading: A tutorial review. In Coltheart, M. (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 12, pp. 327362). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Raney, G. E., & Pollatsek, A. (1995). Eye movements and discourse processing. In Lorch, R. F., and O’Brien, E. J. (Eds.), Sources of coherence in reading (pp. 936). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reder, L. M., & Anderson, J. R. (1982). Effects of spacing and embellishment on memory for the main points of a text. Memory & Cognition, 10, 97102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, P. (2003). Attention and memory during SLA. In Doughty, C., and Long, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 631678). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, J. (2015). Learning second language syntax under massed and distributed conditions. TESOL Quarterly, 49, 857866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H. (2007). Increasing retention without increasing study time. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 183186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, R. J. (1984). Processing time for repetitions and the spacing effect. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie, 38, 537550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, B. H., & Landauer, T. K. (1978). Memory for at least one of two items: Test and failure of several theories of spacing effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 669680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 6377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russo, R., & Mammarella, N. (2002). Spacing effects in recognition memory: When meaning matters. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 4959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 332). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, N. (2008). Review article: Instructed second language vocabulary learning. Language Teaching Research, 12, 329363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuetze, U. (2015). Spacing techniques in second language vocabulary acquisition: Short-term gains vs. long-term memory. Language Teaching Research, 19, 2842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seabrook, R., Brown, G. D., & Solity, J. E. (2005). Distributed and massed practice: From laboratory to classroom. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 107122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M. (1993). Input enhancement in instructed SLA: Theoretical bases. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 165179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaughnessy, J. J., Zimmerman, J., & Underwood, B. J. (1972). Further evidence on the MP-D effect in free-recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sobel, H. S., Cepeda, N. J., & Kapler, I. V. (2011). Spacing effects in real-world classroom vocabulary learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 763767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. (2017). Effects of distributed practice on the proceduralization of morphology. Language Teaching Research, 21, 166188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output in second language learning. In Cook, G., and Seidlhofer, B. (Eds.), Principle and practice in applied linguistics: Studies in honor of H. G. Widdowson (pp. 125144). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thios, S. J., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1976). Effects of repetition as a function of study-phase retrieval. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 529536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toppino, T. C. (1991). The spacing effect in young children’s free recall: Support for automatic-process explanations. Memory & Cognition, 19, 159167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toppino, T. C., & Bloom, L. C. (2002). The spacing effect, free recall, and two-process theory: A closer look. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 437444.Google ScholarPubMed
Van Strien, J. W., Verkoeijen, P. P., Van der Meer, N., & Franken, I. H. A. (2007). Electrophysiological correlates of word repetition spacing: ERP and induced band power old/new effects with massed and spaced repetitions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66, 205214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verkoeijen, P. P., & Bouwmeester, S. (2008). Using latent class modeling to detect bimodality in spacing effect data. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 545555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verkoeijen, P. P., & Delaney, P. F. (2008). Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 3547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verkoeijen, P. P., Rikers, R. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2004). Detrimental influence of contextual change on spacing effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 796800.Google ScholarPubMed
Verkoeijen, P. P., Rikers, R. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2005). Limitations to the spacing effect: Demonstration of an inverted U-shaped relationship between interrepetition spacing and free recall. Experimental Psychology, 52, 257263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2012). Distributing learning over time: The spacing effect in children’s acquisition and generalization of science concepts. Child Development, 83, 11371144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wahlheim, C. N., Dunlosky, J., & Jacoby, L. L. (2011). Spacing enhances the learning of natural concepts: An investigation of mechanisms, metacognition, and aging. Memory & Cognition, 39, 750763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, S. (2007). The effects of repetition on vocabulary knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 28, 4665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitten, W. B., & Bjork, R. A. (1977). Learning from tests: Effects of spacing. Journal of Memory and Language, 16, 465.Google Scholar
Xue, G., Mei, L., Chen, C., Lu, Z., Poldrack, R., & Dong, Q. (2011). Spaced learning enhances subsequent recognition memory by reducing neural repetition suppression. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 16241633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yin, J. C. P., Del Vecchio, M., Zhou, H., & Tully, T. (1995). CREB as a memory modulator: Induced expression of a dCREB2 activator isoform enhances long-term memory in drosophila. Cell, 81, 107115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zechmeister, E. B., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1980). When you know that you know and when you think that you know but you don’t. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 4144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, X., Wang, C., Liu, Q., Xiao, X., Jiang, T., Chen, C., & Xue, G. (2015). Neural mechanisms of the spacing effect in episodic memory: A parallel EEG and fMRI study. Cortex, 69, 7692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, J. (1975). Free recall after self-paced study: A test of the attention explanation of the spacing effect. American Journal of Psychology, 88, 277291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar