Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T02:22:50.341Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Poor readers are not easy to fool: Comprehension of adjectives with exceptional control properties

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Paul Macaruso*
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital
Donald Shankweiler
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories
Brian Byrne
Affiliation:
University of New England, New South Wales, Australia
Stephen Crain
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories
*
Paul Macaruso, MGH Neurolinguistics Laboratory, 101 Merrimac Street, Boston, MA 02114

Abstract

An earlier experiment by Byrne (1981) found that young, poor readers tend to act out sentences containing adjectives with object control, like easy, as though they were adjectives with subject control, like eager. Byrne interpreted this result as evidence that poor readers lag in the acquisition of syntactic knowledge underlying this distinction. However, the possibility that a processing limitation could have contributed to the poor readers' difficulties with objectcontrol adjectives had not been fully explored. In an effort to tease apart these alternatives, we tested comprehension of object-control adjectives in second grade good and poor readers, using both an act-out task and a sentence-picture matching task. Contrary to Byrne's (1981) results, we did not find significant group differences in interpreting object-control adjectives with either task. Reasons for the discrepancy are suggested, and remedies for the pitfalls in designing experiments to assess syntactic knowledge in young children are proposed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bar-Shalom, E., Crain, S. & Shankweile, D. (1993). A comparison of comprehension and production abilities of good and poor readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 197227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. (1981). Deficient syntactic control in poor readers: Is a weak phonetic memory code responsible? Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 201212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, C. S. (1969). acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S. (1987). On performability: Structure and process in language understanding. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 1, 118.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & Shankweiler, D. (1988). Syntactic complexity and reading acquisition. In Davidson, A. & Green, G. M. (Eds.), Linguistic complexity and text comprehension: Read-ability issues reconsidered (pp. 167192). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Shankweiler, D., Macaruso, P. & Bar-Shalom, E. (1990). Working memory and sentence comprehension: Investigations of children with reading disorder. In Vallar, G. & Shallice, T. (Eds.), Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory (pp. 477508). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1970). Children are nice to understand: Surface structure clues for the recovery of a deep structure. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 397408.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1981). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., Satz, P. & Scholes, R. J. (1981). Developmental changes in the linguistic performance correlates of reading achievement. Brain and Language, 13, 7890.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, S. (1980). Psycholinguistic bases of reading disability: A study in sentence comprehension. Unpublished PhD dissertation, City University of New York.Google Scholar
Hamburger, H. & Crain, S. (1984). Acquisition of cognitive compiling. Cognition, 17, 85136.Google Scholar
Macaruso, P., Bar-Shalom, E., Crain, S. & Shankweiler, D. (1989). Comprehension of temporal terms by good and poor readers. Language and Speech, 32, 4567.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., Shankweiler, D. & Smith, S. T. (1984). The association between comprehension of spoken sentences and early reading ability: The role of phonetic representation. Journal of Child Language, 11, 627643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, E. & DiBenedetto, B. (1986). Decoding Skills Test. Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D. & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and reading disorders: A modular approach. Cognition, 24, 139168.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Gorrell, P., & Tuller, B. (1989). Reception of language in Broca's aphasia. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 133.Google Scholar
Smith, S. T., Macaruso, P., Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1989). Syntactic comprehension in young poor readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 429454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. T., Mann, V. A., & Shankweiler, D. (1986). Spoken sentence comprehension by good and poor readers: A study with the Token Test. Cortex, 22, 627632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, C. L., Cairns, H. S., & Zurif, E. B. (1984). Sentence comprehension limitations related to syntactic deficits in reading-disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 305322.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, C. C. (1983). Token Test performance by dyslexic adolescents. Brain and Language, 18, 224235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed