Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T04:53:53.172Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonetic coding and order memory in relation to reading proficiency: A comparison of short-term memory for temporal and spatial order information

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Robert B. Katz*
Affiliation:
Haskins Laboratories and University of Connecticut
Alice F. Healy
Affiliation:
Haskins Laboratories and Yale University
Donald Shankweiler
Affiliation:
Haskins Laboratories and University of Connecticut
*
Robert B. Katz, Haskins Laboratories, 270 Crown St., New Haven, CT 06510.

Abstract

Since children with reading disability are known to have problems using a phonetic memory strategy, it was expected that their recall of order would be inferior to that of good readers in situations where a phonetic strategy is optimal, i.e., when temporal order recall, but not necessarily spatial order recall, is required. On separate tests for retention of temporal sequence and spatial location, the good readers were better than the poor readers on the temporal order task as expected, but contrary to expectation, they maintained their superiority on the spatial task as well. Nevertheless, differences in the error patterns of the good and the poor readers are supportive of earlier evidence that links poor readers' short-term memory deficiencies to reduced effectiveness of phonetic representation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baddeley, A. D.The trouble with levels: A rexamination of Craik and Lockhardt's framework for memory research. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 139152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. Working memory and reading. In Kolers, P. A., Wrolstad, M. E., & Bouma, H. (Eds.), The processing of visible language. New York: Plenum Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Bakker, D. J.Temporal order in disturbed reading. Rotterdam: Rotterdam University, 1972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benton, A. L. Developmental dyslexia: Neurological aspects. In Friedlander, W. J. (Ed.), Advances in neurology (Vol. 7). New York: Raven Press, 1975.Google Scholar
Berch, D. B.Coding of spatial and temporal information in episodic memory. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 1979, 13, 146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Box, G. E. P.Some theories on quadratic forms applied in the study of analysis of variance problems: II. Effects of inequality of variance and covariance between errors in the two-way classification. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 1954, 25, 484498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B., & Shea, P.Semantic and phonetic memory codes in beginning readers. Memory & Cognition. 1979, 7, 333338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P.Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1975.Google Scholar
Corkin, S.Serial-ordering deficits in inferior readers. Neuropsychologia, 1974, 12, 347354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowder, R. G. Language and memory. In Kavanagh, J. F. & Strange, W. (Eds.), Speech and language in the laboratory, school, and clinic. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1978.Google Scholar
Crowder, R. G. Is it just reading? In T. Tighe & B. Schiff (Eds.), Development of perception and cognition, in press.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M.Peabody picture vocabulary test. Circle Pines, Minn.: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1959.Google Scholar
Gates-MacGinitie reading tests (2nd Edition). Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1978.Google Scholar
Healy, A. F.Coding of temporal-spatial patterns in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1975, 14, 481495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, A. F.Pattern coding of spatial order information in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1977, 16, 419437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, A. F.A Markov model for the short-term retention of spatial location information. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1978, 17, 295308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, A. F. Short-term memory for order information. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16). New York: Academic Press, 1982.Google Scholar
Holmes, D. R., & McKeever, W. F.Material specific serial memory deficit in adolescent dyslexics. Cortex, 1979, 15, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, R. S.Phonological coding in dyslexic readers. British Journal of Psychology, 1982, 73, 455460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, R. B., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. Y.Memory for item order and phonetic recoding in the beginning reader. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1981, 32, 474484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimura, D.Right temporal-lobe damage. Archives of Neurology, 1963, 8, 264271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M., Mattingly, I. G., & Turvey, M. T. Language codes and memory codes. In Melton, A. W. & Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory. N.Y.: Wiley, 1972.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A. M., Fowler, C., & Fischer, F. W. Phonetic segmentation and recoding in the beginning reader. In Reber, A. S. & Scarborough, D. (Eds.), Toward a psychology of reading: The proceedings of the CUNY Conference. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D.Children's memory for sentences and word strings in relation to reading ability. Memory & Cognition, 1980, 8, 329335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mark, L. S., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., & Fowler, C. A.Phonetic recoding and reading difficulty in beginning readers. Memory & Cognition, 1977, 5, 623629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, M., & Katz, L.Visual processing of nonlinguistic strings: Redundancy effects and reading ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1976, 105, 338348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, M., Katz, L., & Wicklund, D. A.Immediate spatial order memory and item memory in sixth-grade children as a function of reader ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1975, 67, 610616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noelker, R. W., & Schumsky, D. A.Memory for sequence, form, and position as related to the identification of reading retardates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1973, 64, 2225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, R. K., Davidson, B. J., Kliegl, R., & Davies, S. E. Development of phonetic memory in disabled and normal readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Mark, L. S., Fowler, C. A., & Fischer, F. W.The speech code and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1979, 5, 531545.Google Scholar
Siegel, L. S., & Linder, B. A. Short-term memory processes in children with reading disabilities. Developmental Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
Stanley, G., Kaplan, I., & Poole, C.Cognitive and nonverbal perceptual processing in dyslexics. Journal of General Psychology, 1975, 93, 6772.Google ScholarPubMed
Symmes, J. S., & Rapoport, J. L.Unexpected reading failure. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1972, 42, 8291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J., & Goldman, T.Verbal rehearsal and short-term memory in reading-disabled children. Child Development, 1977, 48, 5660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W.Woodcock reading mastery tests. Circle Pines, Minn.: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1973.Google Scholar