Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T17:12:08.042Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Performance on nonlinguistic visual tasks by children with language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2008

JENNIFER WINDSOR*
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
KATHRYN KOHNERT
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
AMANDA L. LOXTERCAMP
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
PUI-FONG KAN
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Jennifer Windsor, Department of Speech–Language–Hearing Sciences, 115 Shevlin Hall, University of Minnesota, 164 Pillsbury Dr. S.E., Minneapolis, MN 55455. E-mail: windsor@umn.edu

Abstract

The performance of 8- to 13-year-old monolingual English-speaking children with language impairment (LI) on seven nonlinguistic tasks was compared with two groups of typically developing children, monolingual English-speaking children, and proficient Spanish–English sequential bilingual children. Group differences were apparent, with a key finding that the LI group was observably slower than both typical groups in mental rotation and arithmetic, and also slower than the typical monolingual group in odd man out, pattern matching, and form completion. Overall, the response time (RT) increased equivalently across groups as task difficulty increased. Chronological age and perceptual–motor speed contributed to task performance, especially for the shorter tasks. RT trajectories across the 6-year age span showed that task RT decreased with age, but with greater variability for longer tasks that may also be more vulnerable to the effect of experience.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. (1998). Practice parameters for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with language disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 37 (Suppl.), 4962.Google Scholar
Arvedson, P. (2000). Young children with specific language impairment and their numerical cognition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 970–928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, T., & Eysenck, H. (1993). Intelligence, inspection time, and decision time. Intelligence, 17, 523531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, T., & Stough, C. (1998). Improved reaction time method, information processing speed, and intelligence. Intelligence, 26, 5362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernardo, A. (2002). Language and mathematical problem solving among bilinguals. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 136, 283297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636644.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 6985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birren, J., & Fisher, L. (1995). Aging and speed of behavior: Possible consequences for psychological functioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 329353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. (1990). Handedness, clumsiness and developmental language disorders. Neuropsychologia, 28, 681690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Specific language impairment as a maturational lag: Evidence from longitudinal data on language and motor development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 442459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R., & Johnsen, S. (1997). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence—III (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Burns, N., & Nettelbeck, T. (2003). Inspection time in the structure of cognitive abilities: Where does IT fit? Intelligence, 31, 237255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Needleman, H., & Janosky, J. (1997). Reducing bias in language assessment: Processing dependent measures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 519525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerella, J., & Hale, S. (1994). The rise and fall in information-processing rates over the life span. Acta Psychologica, 86, 109197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chelazzi, L. (1999). Serial attention mechanisms in visual search: A critical look at the evidence. Psychological Research, 62, 195219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, R., Donlan, C., Newton, E., & Lloyd, D. (2005). Number skills and knowledge in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 732744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Courbois, Y. (2000). The role of stimulus axis salience in children's ability to mentally rotate unfamiliar figures. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 261269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284, 970974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diascro, M., & Brody, N. (1994). Odd-man-out and intelligence. Intelligence, 19, 7992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C. (1998). Spoken word recognition in children with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 193207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donlan, C., & Gourlay, S. (1999). The importance of non-verbal skills in the acquisition of place-value knowledge: Evidence from normally-developing and language-impaired children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. (1999). An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12491260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis Weismer, S., & Hesketh, L. (1996). Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 177190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. (1994). The counting abilities of children with specific language impairment: A comparison of oral and gestural tasks. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 358368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. (1996). Mathematical abilities of children with specific language impairment: A 2-year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 839849.Google ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. (1998). The effect of presentation rate on serial memory in young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 13751383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. (1999). Arithmetic calculation, short-term memory, and language performance in children with specific language performance: A 5-year follow-up. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 420431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frearson, W., & Eysenck, H. (1986). Intelligence, reaction time (RT) and a new “odd-man-out” RT paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 807817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geary, D., Cormier, P., Goggin, J., Estrada, P., & Lunn, M. (1993). Mental arithmetic: A componential analysis of speed-of-processing across monolingual, weak bilingual, and strong bilingual adults. International Journal of Psychology, 28, 185201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillam, R., Cowan, N., & Marler, J. (1998). Information processing by school-age children with specific language impairment: Evidence from a modality effect paradigm. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 913926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B., Fabiano, L., & Washington, P. (2005). Phonological skills in predominantly English-speaking, predominantly Spanish-speaking, and Spanish–English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 201218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Håkansson, G., & Nettelbladt, U. (1996). Similarities between SLI and L2 children: Evidence from the acquisition of Swedish word order. In Johnson, C. & Gilbert, J. (Eds.), Children's language (Vol. 9, pp. 135151). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hick, R., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Cognitive abilities in children with specific language impairment: Consideration of visuo-spatial skills. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 137149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, E. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 149171.Google ScholarPubMed
Hill, E., & Bishop, D. (1998). A reaching test reveals weak hand preference in specific language impairment and developmental coordination disorder. Laterality, 3, 295310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, L., & Gillam, R. (2004). Verbal and spatial information processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 114125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four-Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Department of Sociology.Google Scholar
Johnston, J., & Ellis Weismer, S. (1983). Mental rotation abilities in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 397403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R. (1988). Developmental functions for speeds of cognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45, 339364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R. (1991a). Controlled and automatic processing during mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 337347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R. (1991b). Processing time declines exponentially during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 259266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1993). Processing time decreases globally at an exponential rate during childhood and adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 254265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R. (1994). A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 418421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamhi, A., Catts, H., Mauer, D., Apel, K., & Gentry, B. (1988). Phonological and spatial processing abilities in language- and reading-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 316327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, B., Dewey, D., Crawford, S., & Wilson, B. (2001). The term comorbidity is of questionable value in reference to developmental disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 555565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Atypical epigenesist. Developmental Science, 10, 8488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Thomas, M., Annaz, D., Humphreys, K., Ewing, S., Brace, N., et al. (2004). Exploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: The importance of building developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 12581274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohnert, K., & Windsor, J. (2004). The search for common ground: Part II. Nonlinguistic performance by linguistically diverse learners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 891903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Miller, R. (2004). Crossing borders: Recognition of Spanish words by English-speaking children with and without language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 543564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Yim, D. (2006). Do language-based processing tasks separate children with primary language impairment from typical bilinguals? Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 21, 1929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kranzler, J. (1994). Application of the techniques of mental chronometry to the study of learning disabilities. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 853859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kranzler, J., Whang, P., & Jensen, A. (1994). Task complexity and the speed and efficiency of elemental information processing: Another look at the nature of intellectual giftedness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 447459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, M., & Edwards, J. (1996). Why do children with specific language impairment name pictures more slowly than their peers? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 10811097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. (1987). Is specific language impairment a useful construct? In Rosenberg, S. (Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics (pp. 139). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Likert, R., & Quahsa, W. (1970). Revised Minnesota Paper Form Board Test. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M., & Christiansen, M. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 3554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mägiste, F. (1980). Memory for numbers in monolinguals and bilinguals. Psychological Research, 42, 363373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeay, H. (2003). The relationship between bilingualism and the performance of spatial tasks. Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 6, 423438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahon, W. (1999). Advances in the cognitive neuroscience of neurodevelopmental disorders: Views from child psychiatry and medical genetics. In Tager-Flusberg, H. (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 583600). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Miller, C., Kail, R., Leonard, L., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, C., Leonard, L., Kail, R., Zhang, X., Tombin, J. B., & Francis, D. (2006). Response time in 14-year-olds with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 712--728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. (2000). Relation of working memory to off-line and real-time sentence processing in children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 117148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J., & Leonard, L. (1998). Real-time inflectional processing by children with specific language impairment: Effects of phonetic substance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 14321443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mruczek, R., & Sheinberg, D. (2005). Distractor familiarity leads to more efficient visual search for complex stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 10161031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 360386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). French–English bilingual children with SLI: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 113127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powell, R., & Bishop, D. (1992). Clumsiness and perceptual problems in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 34, 755765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodekohr, R., & Haynes, W. (2001). Differentiating dialect from disorder: A comparison of two processing tasks and a standardized language test. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 255272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosser, R. (1994). Children's solution strategies and mental rotation problems: The differential salience of stimulus components. Child Study Journal, 24, 153168.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. (1993). Speed and knowledge as determinants of adult age differences in verbal tasks. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, P29P36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T., & Hedden, T. (2002). Interpreting reaction time measures in between-group comparisons. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 858872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, R., & Frederickson, N. (2005). Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid automatic naming of digits and text-reading speed. Brain and Language, 93, 152159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savich, P. (1984). Anticipatory imagery ability in normal and language-disabled children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 494501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1997). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (Spanish ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Do autism and specific language impairment represent overlapping language disorders? In Rice, M. L. & Warren, S. F. (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 3152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Takano, Y. (1989). Perception of rotated forms: A theory of information types. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomblin, B. J. (2006). A normativist account of language-based learning disability. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 21, 818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, B. J., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 12451260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M., & Pierpoint, (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windsor, J., & Hwang, M. (1999). Children's auditory lexical decisions: A limited processing account of language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 9901002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windsor, J., & Kohnert, K. (2004). The search for common ground: Part I. Lexical performance by linguistically diverse learners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 877890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windsor, J., & Kohnert, K. (in press). Processing measures of cognitive–linguistic interactions for children with language and reading disorders. In Mody, M. & Silliman, E. (Eds.), Language impairment and reading disability: Brain, behavior, and experience. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., Milbrath, R., Carney, E., & Rakowski, S. (2001). General slowing in language impairment: Methodological considerations in testing the hypothesis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 446461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed