Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T07:01:07.189Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuroplasticity as a model for bilingualism: Commentary on Baum and Titone

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2014

Natalie A. Phillips*
Affiliation:
Concordia University

Extract

Un grand merci à Shari Baum and Debra Titone for their review paper, “Moving Toward a Neuroplasticity View of Bilingualism, Executive Control and Aging.” (Please note the code switch in that previous sentence is part of my effort to build my cognitive reserve.) The authors are to be congratulated for providing a timely and constructive review of the current state of our understanding of the potential impact of bilingualism on executive control processes and cognitive aging. In this short commentary, I will comment on one of their key contributions to the discussion, namely, that researchers should embrace and address the individual variability among bilinguals.

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959963. doi:10.1002/ana.24158 Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., et al. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24, 4049.Google Scholar
Chertkow, H., Whitehead, V., Phillips, N., Wolfson, C., Atherton, J., & Bergman, H. (2010). Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: Evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 24, 118125.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M. (2010). Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 75, 17261729.Google Scholar
Duncan, H. D., & Phillips, N. A. (in press). The contribution of bilingualism to cognitive reserve in healthy aging and dementia. In Nicoladis, E. & Montanari, S. (Eds.), Lifespan perspectives on bilingualism. Washington, DC: APA Books.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (2011). Language control in different contexts: The behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontier in Psychology, 2, 103.Google Scholar
Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23, 7078. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.70 Google Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012a). Bilingualism and cognitive control in healthy older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 45 (Suppl. 1), 5960.Google Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012b). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 65, 356369.Google Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012c). Conflict monitoring and resolution: Are two languages better than one? Evidence from reaction time and event-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 1446, 7190.Google Scholar