Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:32:56.115Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Morphological awareness and word-level reading in early and middle elementary school years

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 May 2019

Erin K. Robertson*
Affiliation:
Cape Breton University
S. Hélène Deacon
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: erin_robertson@cbu.ca

Abstract

We examined whether morphological awareness made a significant contribution to word-level reading across Grades 1 to 4. We test these relations specifically in a task measuring awareness of past-tense forms. A total of 375 children from Grades 1 to 4 completed tasks assessing past-tense morphological awareness along with real word and pseudoword reading. Children also completed control measures assessing phonological awareness, phonological short-term memory, sentence-level language skills, and nonverbal cognitive ability. After these controls, past-tense morphological awareness was a significant predictor of real word reading in Grades 1 and 2, but not in Grades 3 and 4. Further, following on all controls, past-tense morphological awareness was a consistent predictor of pseudoword reading across Grades 1 to 4. Morphological awareness, at least as measured with past-tense verbs, appears to have a role in word reading across the early to middle elementary school grades; for young readers, there are relations to reading of both known and novel words, and for older readers, relations are significant specifically in reading novel words. These findings are discussed within the context of theories of word reading development.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Apel, K., Brimo, D., Diehm, E., & Apel, L. (2013). Morphological awareness intervention with kindergarten, first, and second grade students from low SES homes: A feasibility study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 44, 161173. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/12-0042)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M. D., Joanisse, M. F., & Shepherd, M. (2008). Associations between key language-related measures in typically developing school-aged children. Journal of Psychology, 216, 161171.Google Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Growth in phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness in grades 1 to 6. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 141163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80, 144179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read—A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brittain, M. M. (1970). Inflectional performance and early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 6, 3448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological awareness on literacy achievement: An integrative review. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 464487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Kearns, D. M. (2017). Learning to read morphologically complex words. In Cain, K., Compton, D. L., & Parilla, R. K. (Eds.), Theories of reading development. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J., & Nomanbhoy, D. M. (1993). Phonological and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casalis, S., & Louis-Alexandre, M. F. (2000). Morphological analysis, phonological analysis, and learning to read French: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing, 12, 303335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 13781396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Census Canada. (2016). Statistics Canada. Retrieved from https://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/index-eng.cfmGoogle Scholar
Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Dawson, N., Rastle, K., & Ricketts, J. (2018). Morphological effects in visual word recognition: Children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 645654.Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H. (2012). Sounds, letters, and meanings: The independent influences of phonological, morphological, and orthographic skills on early word reading accuracy. Journal of Research in Reading, 35, 456475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., & Bryant, P. (2006). Getting to the root: Young writers’ sensitivity to the role of root morphemes in the spelling of inflected and derived words. Journal of Child Language, 33, 401. doi: 10.1017/s0305000906007409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Tong, X., & Francis, K. (2017). The relationship of morphological analysis and morphological decoding to reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 40, 116. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12056CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Wade-Woolley, L., & Kirby, J. R. (2007). Crossover: The role of morphological awareness in French immersion children’s reading. Developmental Psychology, 43, 732746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derwing, B. L., & Baker, W. J. (1979). Recent research on the acquisition of English morphology. In Fletcher, P., & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (pp. 209223). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Derwing, B. L., & Baker, W. J. (1986). Assessing morphological development. In Fletcher, P. J., & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition: Studies in first language development (2nd ed., pp. 326338). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 116125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (2014). Orthographic mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling, memory, and vocabulary learning. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, J., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2008). Contribution of phonological and broader language skills to literacy. International Journal of Communication Disorders, 43, 552569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of morphological interventions in English: Effects on literacy outcomes for school-age children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 257285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonnerman, L. M., Seidenberg, M. S., & Anderson, E. S. (2007). Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: Evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 323345. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, M. H., Penney, S. B., & Robertson, E. K. (2017). The effects of phonological short-term memory and speech perception on spoken sentence comprehension in children: Simulating deficits in an experimental design. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 12131235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joshi, M., Binks, E., Hougen, M., Dahlgren, M. E., Ocker-Dean, E., & Smith, D. L. (2009). Why elementary teachers might be inadequately prepared to teach reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 392402. doi: 10.1177/0022219409338736CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (2nd ed.). Bloomington, MN: Pearson.Google Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., Petscher, Y., Proctor, C. P., & Silverman, R. D. (2016). Is the whole greater than the sum of its parts? Modeling the contributions of language comprehension skills to reading comprehension in the upper elementary grades. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20, 436454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruck, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 1034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levesque, K. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Inferring meaning from meaningful parts: The contributions of morphological skills to the development of children’s reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 54(1), 6380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahony, D., Singson, M., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity to morphological relations. Reading and Writing, 12, 191218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mota, M. M. P. E., Freitas Junior, P. V., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507525. doi: 10.1017/ S0142716417000479Google Scholar
Nagy, W., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W., Berringer, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Diabkidoy, I. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1993). The acquisition of morphology: Learning the contribution of the suffixes to the meaning of derivations. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 155170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quémart, P., Gonnerman, L. M., Downing, J., & Deacon, S. H. (2017). The development of morphological representations in young readers: A cross-modal priming study. Developmental Science, 21, e12607. doi: 10.1111/desc.12607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabin, J., & Deacon, S. H. (2008). The representation of morphologically complex words in the developing lexicon. Journal of Child Language, 35, 453465. doi: 10.1017/S0305000907008525CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rispens, J. E., McBride-Chang, C., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Morphological awareness and advanced word recognition and spelling in Dutch. Reading and Writing, 21, 587607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, E. K., Joanisse, M. F., Desroches, A. S., & Ng, S. (2009). Categorical speech perception deficits distinguish language and reading impairments in children. Developmental Science, 12, 753767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, E. K., Joanisse, M. F., Desroches, A. S., & Terry, A. (2013). Past-tense morphology and phonological deficits in children with dyslexia and children with language impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46, 230240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., Wade-Woolley, L., & Deacon, S. H. (2009). Toward a comprehensive view of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 96113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothou, K. M., & Padeliadu, S. (2014). Inflectional morphological awareness and word reading and reading comprehension in Greek. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 10071027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruan, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Song, S., Li, Y., & Shu, H. (2018). Does writing system influence the associations between phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and reading? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 180202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & Gonnerman, L. M. (2000). Explaining derivational morphology as the convergence of codes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 353361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. H. (2005). Early reading development in European orthographies. In Snowling, M. J., and Hulme, C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 296315). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (2010). Academic language and the challenge of reading. Science, 5977, 328.Google Scholar
Spear-Swerling, L., & Brucker, P. (2005). Teachers’ literacy-related knowledge and self-perceptions in relation to preparation and experience. Annals of Dyslexia, 55, 332364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics (6th ed.). Boston: Pearson.Google Scholar
Taft, M. (2004). Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 745765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Cassar, M., & Zukowski, A. (1994). What types of linguistic information do children use in spelling? The case of flaps. Child Development, 65, 13181337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2014). How children learn to write words. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 7387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Windsor, J. (1994). Children’s comprehension and production of derivational suffixes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 408417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W. (2011). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests—Revised Normative Update. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar