Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-8kt4b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-24T04:22:32.995Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interacting processes and developmental biases allow learners to crack the “what” code and the “who” code in spoken language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2018

Linda Polka
Affiliation:
School of Communication Sciences & Disorders and Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music McGill University
Thierry Nazzi
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie de la Perception (UMR8242) and Université Paris Descartes

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Commentaries
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Benavides-Varela, S., Hochmann, J.-R., Macagno, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (2012). Newborn’s brain activity signals the origin of word memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 109, 1790817913.Google Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 113, 1239712402.Google Scholar
Bouchon, C., Floccia, C., Fux, T., Adda-Decker, M., & Nazzi, T. (2015). Call me Alix, not Elix: Vowels are more important than consonants in own name recognition at 5 months. Developmental Science, 18, 587598.Google Scholar
Bundgaard-Nielsen, R. L., Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2011a). Vocabulary size matters: The assimilation of second-language Australian English vowels to first-language Japanese vowel categories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 5167.Google Scholar
Bundgaard-Nielsen, R. L., Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2011b). Vocabulary size is associated with second-language vowel perception performance in adult learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 433461.Google Scholar
Hochmann, J.-R., Benavides-Varela, S., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (2011). Consonants and vowels: Different roles in early language acquisition. Developmental Science, 14, 14451458.Google Scholar
Houston, D. M., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 15701582.Google Scholar
Johnson, E. K., Westrek, E., Nazzi, T., & Cutler, A. (2011). Infant ability to tell voices apart rests on language experience. Developmental Science, 14, 10021011.Google Scholar
Kadam, M. A., Orena, A. J., Theodore, R. M., & Polka, L. (2016). Reading ability influences native and non-native voice recognition, even for unimpaired readers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139, EL6EL12.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a “perceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 93107.Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Bertoncini, J., Barrière, M., & Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D. (1978). Infant recognition of mother’s voice. Perception, 7, 491497.Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143178.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T. (2005). Use of phonetic specificity during the acquisition of new words: Differences between consonants and vowels. Cognition, 98, 1330.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., & Bertoncini, J. (2009). Consonant specificity in onset and coda positions in early lexical acquisition. Language and Speech, 52, 463480.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 756766.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., & Polka, L. (2018). The consonant bias in word learning is not determined by position within the word: Evidence from vowel-initial words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 174, 103111.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Poltrock, S., & von Holzen, K. (2016). The developmental origins of the consonant bias in lexical processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 291296.Google Scholar
Nespor, M., Peña, M., & Mehler, J. (2003). On the different roles of vowels and consonants in speech processing and language acquisition. Lingue E Linguaggio, 2, 203229.Google Scholar
Nishibayashi, L.-L., & Nazzi, T. (2016). Vowels then consonants: Emergence of a consonant bias in early word form segmentation. Cognition, 155, 188203.Google Scholar
Orena, A. J., Theodore, R., & Polka, L. (2015). Language exposure facilitates talker learning prior to language comprehension, even in adults. Cognition, 143, 3640.Google Scholar
Perrachione, T. K., Del Tufo, S. N., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Human voice recognition depends on language ability. Science, 333, 595.Google Scholar
Polka, L., & Werker, J. F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 421435.Google Scholar
Poltrock, S., & Nazzi, T. (2015). Consonant/vowel asymmetry in early word form recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1312, 135148.Google Scholar
Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science, 10, 172175.Google Scholar
Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2012). Six-month-olds comprehend words that refer to parts of the body. Infancy, 17, 432444.Google Scholar
von Holzen, K., Rider, D., & Nazzi, T. (2017). Consonant and vowel processing in 5-, 8-, and 11-month-olds own name recognition: The role of acoustic/phonetic and lexical factors, Poster presented at the 14th International Congress for the Study of Child Language (IASCL): Lyon, France July 17–21, 2017.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 7, 4963.Google Scholar