Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-v5vhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T00:45:06.815Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The acquisition of object names in children with specific language impairment: Action context and word extension

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Richard G. Schwartz*
Affiliation:
Purdue University
Laurence B. Leonard
Affiliation:
Purdue University
Cheryl Messick
Affiliation:
Children's Hospital, Cuyahoga Falls, OH
Kathy Chapman
Affiliation:
Case Western University
*
Richard G. Schwartz, Audiology & Speech Sciences, Heavilon Hall, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907

Abstract

Specifically-language-impaired children and younger normal children matched for expressive language were presented with unfamiliar object names and referents across five experimental sessions. The objects differed in the degree to which they were associated with actions, and only certain of the object exemplars were named during presentation. Comprehension testing revealed that the specifically-language-impaired children acquired a greater number of object concepts presented in a no-action condition than the normal children. However, their extension of the names to new exemplars was more restricted and less differentiated. Several possible accounts of these findings are evaluated.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Arthur, G. (1952). The Arthur Adaptation of the Leiter International Performance Scale. Chicago: Stoelting.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. (1983). The early acquisition and development of the meanings of action-related words. In Seiler, T. & Wannenmacher, W. (Eds.), Concept development and the development of word meaning (pp. 191209). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. (1986). Early semantic representations and early word-usage. In Kuczaj, S. & Barrett, M. (Eds.), The development of word meaning (pp. 3967). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1977). The structure and origin of semantic categories in the language learning child. Paper presented at Symposium on Fundamentals of Symbolism,Burg Wartenstein, Austria.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1978). The acquisition of word meaning: An investigation into some current conflicts. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (Eds.), The development of communication (pp. 263288). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Chapman, K., Leonard, L., Rowan, L., & Weiss, A. (1983). Inappropriate word extensions in the speech of young language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 5562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fivush, R., & Slackman, E. (1986). The acquisition and development of scripts. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fremgen, A., & Fay, D. (1980). Overextensions in production and comprehension: A methodological clarification. Journal of Child Language, 7, 205211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, S., & Taylor, M. (1984). How two-year-old children interpret proper and common names for unfamiliar objects. Child Development, 55, 15351540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, J., & Kuczaj, S. (1982). Towards a theory of substantive word-meaning acquisition. In Kuczaj, S. (Ed.), Language development. Volume 1: Syntax and semantics (pp. 275311). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Irwin, J., Ward, M., Greis, A., Deen, C., Cooley, V., & Auvenshine, A. (1974). The Lexington Developmental Scale. Lexington, KY: The Child Development Centers of United Cerebral Palsy of the Bluegrass.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. (1982). Interpreting the Leiter IQ: Performance profiles of young normal and language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 291296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuczaj, S. (1986). Thoughts on the intensional basis of early object word extension: Evidence from comprehension and production. In Kuczaj, S. & Barrett, M. (Eds.), The development of word meaning (pp. 99120). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lederberg, A. (1980). The language environment of children with language delays. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 5, 141159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. (in press). Is specific language impairment a useful construct? In Rosenberg, S. (Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics. Volume 1: Disorders of first-language development.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., & Schwartz, R. (1985). Early linguistic development in children with specific language impairment. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's language. Volume 5 (pp. 291318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Schwartz, R., Chapman, K., Rowan, L., Prelock, P., Terrell, B., Weiss, A., & Messick, C. (1982). Early lexical acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 554564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lock, A. (1980). The guided reinvention of language. London: Academic.Google Scholar
Lucariello, J., Kyratzis, A., & Engel, S. (1986). Event representations, context, and language. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 137159). Hills-dale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Merriman, W. (1986). Some reasons for the occurrence and eventual correction of children's naming errors. Child Development, 57, 942952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1983). The conceptual basis for language. In Seiler, T. B. & Wannenmacher, W. (Eds.), Concept development and the development of word meaning (pp. 173188). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: The acquisition of shared meaning. Orlando, FL: Academic.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., & Bonvillian, J. (1973). Concepts and words in the 18-month-old: Acquiring concept names under controlled conditions. Cognition, 2, 435450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. E., & Bonvillian, J. (1978). Early language development: Conceptual growth and related processes between 2½ and 4 years of age. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's language (pp. 467556). New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Olswang, L., Bain, B., Dunn, C., & Cooper, J. (1983). The effects of stimulus variation on lexical learning. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 192201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrey, S. (1977). Word associations and the development of lexical memory. Cognition, 5, 5771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., & Mervis, D. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Brwem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, G., Nelson, K., Wetstone, H., & Tanouye, E. (1986). Acquisition and generalization of novel object concepts by young language learners. Journal of Child Language, 13, 6783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, J., & Chapman, R. (1977). Who is “Daddy” revisited: The status of two-year-olds' overextended words in use and comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 4, 359375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoss, K., & Darley, F. (1974). Developmental apraxia of speech in children with defective articulation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 17, 399416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, I., Steiner, V., & Evatt, R. (1969). Preschool Language Scale. Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar