Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T07:59:58.044Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's narrative productions: A comparison of personal event and fictional stories

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Marybeth S. Allen*
Affiliation:
John R. Graham School and University of Maine
Marilyn K. Kertoy
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
John C. Sherblom
Affiliation:
University of Maine
John M. Pettit
Affiliation:
University of Maine
*
Marybeth S. Allen, 114 Main Street, Orono, ME 04473

Abstract

Personal event narratives and fictional stories are narrative genres which emerge early and undergo further development throughout the preschool and early elementary school years. This study compares personal event and fictional narratives across two language-ability groups using episodic analysis. Thirty-six normal children (aged 4 to 8 years) were divided into high and low language-ability groups using Developmental Sentence Scoring (DSS). Three fictional stories and three personal event narratives were gathered from each subject and were scored for length in communication units, total types of structures found within the narrative, and structure of the whole narrative. Narrative genre differences significantly influenced narrative structure for both language-ability groups and narrative length for the high language-ability group. Personal events were told with more reactive sequences and complete episodes than fictional stories, while fictional stories were told with more action sequences and multiple-episode structures. Compared to the episodic story structure of fictional stories, where a prototypical ‘good” story is a multiple-episode structure, a reactive sequence and/or a single complete episode structure may be an alternate, involving mature narrative forms for relating personal events. These findings suggest that narrative structures for personal event narratives and fictional stories may follow different developmental paths. Finally, differences in productive language abilities contributed to the distinctions in narrative structure between fictional stories and personal event narratives. As compared to children in the low group, children in the high group told narratives with greater numbers of complete and multiple episodes, and their fictional stories were longer than their personal event narratives.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Apel, K., Deem, J. F. & Phelps, T. A. (1989). Generating and recalling narratives in scripted and non-scripted conditions and immediate and delayed narrative recall in scripted and non-scripted stories. Paper presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, St. Louis, MO.Google Scholar
Berman, R. & Slobin, D. (1986). Coding manual: Temporality in discourse (rev. ed.). Cognitive Science Program, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Blank, M., Rose, S. A., and Berlin, L. J. (1978). The language of learning: The preschool years. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Cook-Gumperz, J. (1977). Situated instructions: Language socialization of school age children. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.), Child discourse (pp. 103121). New York: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. (1991). Teacher agenda and setting: Constraints on conversation in preschools. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 225301). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
DLM Teaching Resources. (1984). What's wrong here? (rev.) (Catalog #F9400). DLM Teaching Resources, One DLM Park, Allen, TX, 75002.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in conversation. Discourse Processes, 8, 177204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., Gray, J. T. & Fromhoff, F. A. (1987). Two-year olds talk about the past. Cognitive Development, 2, 393410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R. & Slackman, E. (1986). The acquisition and development of scripts. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 7196). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
French, L. A. (1986). The language of events. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 119136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gerhardt, J. (1989). Monologue as a speech genre. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 171230). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Glenn, C. & Stein, N. (1980). Syntactic structures and real world themes in stories generated by children (Technical Report). Urbana: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. Language and Society, 11, 4976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedberg, N. L. (1989). Narrative intervention: Enhancing children's oral and written language abilities. Short course materials presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Convention, St. Louis, MO.Google Scholar
Hedberg, N. L. & Fink, R. J. (1985). Surface and deep structure characteristics of language disordered children's narratives. Paper presented at the American Speech-Language- Hearing Association Convention, Washington. DC.Google Scholar
Hedberg, N. L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (1986). Narrative analysis: Clinical procedures. Topic in Language Disorders, 7, 5869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hicks, D. (1990). Narrative skills and genre knowledge: Ways of telling in the primary school grades. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 83104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. A. (1986). Memories are made of this: General event knowledge and the development of autobiographical memory. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 87118). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hudson, J. A. & Nelson, K. (1986). Repeated encounters of a similar kind: Effects of familiarity on children's autobiographical memory. Cognitive Development, 1, 232271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. A. & Shapiro, L. R. (1991). From knowing to telling: The development of children's scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 89136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hutson-Nechkash, P. (1991). Storybuilding: A guide to structuring oral narratives. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.Google Scholar
Kemper, S. (1984). The development of narrative skills: Explanations and entertainments. In Kuczaj, S. A. (Ed.), Discourse development (pp. 99123). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, D. F. & Goodman, D. S. (1990). Whole language: Cherishing learners and their language. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 21, 221227.Google Scholar
Klecan-Aker, J. S., Mclngvale, G. K. & Swank, P. (1987). Stimulus considerations in narrative analysis of normal 3rd grade children. Language and Speech, 30, 1324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in Black English vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Lahey, M. (1988). Language development into the school years: Further goals. In Lahey, M. (Ed.), Language disorders and language development (pp. 266288). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Lee, L. L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. & Johnson, N. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M. (1967). A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York: Dutton, Dial Books for Young Readers.Google Scholar
McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (1983). A naturalistic study of the production of causal connectives by children. Journal of Child Language, 12, 145159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the story: A longitudinal study of parental styles in eliciting narratives and developing narrative skill. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 217253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Merritt, D. D., & Liles, B. Z. (1987). Story grammar ability in children with and without language disorder: Story generation, story retelling, and story comprehension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 539552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merritt, D. D., & Liles, B. Z. (1989). Narrative analysis: Clinical applications of story generation and story retelling. Journal of Speech and Language Disorders, 54, 438447.Google ScholarPubMed
Michaels, S. (1981). “Sharing time”: Children's narrative styles and differential access to literacy. Language in Society, 10, 423442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milosky, L. M. (1987). Narratives in the classroom. Seminars in Speech and Language, 8, 329343.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge and cognitive development. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 87118). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. & Gruendel, J. M. (1986). Children's scripts. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 2146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Norris, J. A. & Damico, J. S. (1990). Whole language in theory and practice: Implications for language intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 21, 212220.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Press & Peel. (1985). Uniset. Roskilde, Denmark: Fair-Print, AS.Google Scholar
Ripich, D. N., & Griffith, P. L. (1988). Narrative abilities of children with learning disabilities and nondisabled children: Story structure, cohesion, and propositions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 165173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, F. P. (1986). Oral narrative abilities of learning-disabled students. Topics in Language Disorders, 7(1), 2130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1986). Narrative discourse: Spontaneously generated stories of learning disabled and normally achieving students. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, A. (1980). A theoretical taxonomy of the differences between oral and written language. In Spiro, R. J., Bruce, B. C., & Brewer, W. F. (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 411439). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
SAS Institute, Inc. (1985). SAS User's guide: Basic version (5th ed.). Carey, NC: SAS Institute, Inc.Google Scholar
Scollon, R., & Scollon, S. B. K. (1981). Narrative, literacy and face in interethnic communication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slackman, E. A., Hudson, J. A., & Fivush, R. (1986). Actions, actors, links, and goals: The structure of children's event representations. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 4769). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sleight, C., & Prinz, P. (1985). Use of abstracts, orientations, and codas in narration by language disordered and nondisordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 361371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53(2), 165189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SPSS, Inc. (1983). SPSS user's guide. Chicago: SPSS-X Inc.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of children's storytelling skill. In Franklin, M. B., & Barten, S. S. (Eds.), Child language: A reader (pp. 282297). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. O. (Ed.), New directions in discourse processes (pp. 53119). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Policastro, M. (1984). The concept of story: A comparison between children's and teacher's viewpoints. In Mandl, H., Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 113159). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stemach, G., & Williams, W. B. (1988). Word express. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Stephens, M. L., Dallman, S., & Montgomery, J. K. (1988). Developmental scoring through age nine. Paper presented at the 1988 American Speech and Hearing Association Convention, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Torrance, N., & Olson, D. R. (1984). Oral language competence and the acquisition of literacy. In Pellegrini, A. D., & Yawkey, T. D. (Eds.), The development of oral and written language in social contexts (pp. 167182). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Westby, C. (1985). Learning to talk: Talking to learn: Oral–literate language differences. In Simon, C. S. (Ed.), Communication skills and classroom success: Therapy methodologies for language-learning disabled students (pp. 181213). San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
White, H. (1980). The value of narrativity in the representation of reality. In Mitchell, W. J. T. (Ed.), On narrative (pp. 149). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar