Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T00:31:32.572Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Review: Individual variability in feeding behaviour of domesticated ruminants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 August 2018

H. W. Neave
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, CanadaV6T 1Z4
D. M. Weary
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, CanadaV6T 1Z4
M. A. G. von Keyserlingk*
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, CanadaV6T 1Z4
*
E-mail: nina@mail.ubc.ca

Abstract

Individual animals behave differently from one another, especially when confronting challenges such as changes in diet (e.g. weaning), environment (e.g. moving from pasture to feedlot) and social grouping (e.g. movement to lactating group after parturition). Each of these challenges involves some element of novelty, impacting the welfare and productivity of the animal. Indeed, the large individual variability in the development and expression of feeding behaviour cannot be fully explained by differences in genetics, management practices, body size or growth rate. In this review we outline evidence that individual variability in feeding behaviour is associated with the personality of the individual. We focus on three key personality traits: exploration, fear or reactivity and sociability. Individuals differ in how much they explore their feeding environment, with more exploratory individuals being less reactive to novel situations. Feeding behaviour can be impaired in individuals that are especially reactive to a change in their environment, change in diet or handling or restraint by humans. The social environment is also a major factor affecting how individuals express their behaviour. Sociability of the individual, including dominant-subordinate and affiliative relationships, affects how individuals make foraging decisions, gain access to feed and adopt particular social strategies to maintain or adjust feeding patterns when the social environment changes. Personality traits such as exploration, boldness and sociability also affect the use of social information when learning where, how or what to eat. Our review highlights the implications of feeding behaviour variability for the welfare and productivity of the individual, and how an understanding of personality can help tailor management to the needs of the individual.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
© The Animal Consortium 2018 

Implications

Individual animals respond differently to stressful events. Personality can influence how the animal responds to stressors, including those associated with the feeding environment. Understanding how personality traits affect feeding behaviour can help to create tailored management that better meets the needs of different individuals on the farm. Personality differences may affect, for example, diet choice in grazing systems, social dynamics in confined systems, and how well animals cope with the various dietary and environmental transitions common on many farms.

Introduction

Individuals vary in their feeding behaviour, including in the expression of distinct and consistent feeding patterns (e.g. Melin et al., Reference Meagher, Weary and von Keyserlingk2005). These feeding patterns develop from an early age (Provenza and Balph, Reference Provenza and Burritt1987), and are influenced by a number of management factors. For instance, the availability of preferred forages or habitats, the frequency of pasture rotation and the distribution and quality of forage sources influence the feeding behaviour of grazing herds (cattle: Launchbaugh and Howery, Reference Laland2005; goats: Goetsch et al., Reference Gibbons, Lawrence and Haskell2010; sheep: Rutter, Reference Sárová, Spinka, Arias Panamá and Sime Cek2006). In confined systems, the timing and frequency of feed delivery, and feed bunk structure and space allowance, are important features of the feeding environment that affect the feeding behaviour of ruminants (cattle: von Keyserlingk and Weary, Reference Huzzey, Fregonesi, von Keyserlingk and Weary2010; goats: Jørgensen et al., Reference Huzzey, DeVries, Valois and von Keyserlingk2007). Domesticated ruminants are generally social so social interactions between group mates, including competition for resources and learning from social partners, can also affect feeding behaviour (Forbes and Kyriazakis, Reference Escós, Alados and Boza1995; Proudfoot and Habing, Reference Provenza2015).

Farm animals often experience changes in nutritional, physical and social aspects of their feeding environment, such as diet, feeding space available for each individual and social regrouping changes. Individuals within a group may cope differently with these management changes. For instance, some individuals may fail or take longer to learn where, how or what to eat; whereas, others may be unable to cope within their social environment and thus fail to gain access to food resources. Both situations have the potential to result in animals that do not achieve their growth potential, perhaps due to inappropriate behavioural responses to the environment that may be accompanied by a pervasive negative emotional state such as frustration, anxiety or panic. Individuals that are more resilient when confronted with these management changes may be more likely to succeed on farms.

There is limited understanding of why individuals within a herd differ in their feeding behaviour, whether these differences are stable, and how these may vary in grazing v. confined systems. Knowing why characteristic feeding patterns develop and persist may help tailor management to the needs of the individual, especially given the development of technologies (e.g. precision nutrition) that allow for management at the individual rather than herd level. Behaviours that are consistent within individuals, but vary between individuals, are indicative of ‘personality’. Specific aspects of the behavioural repertoire are referred to as personality ‘traits’, using terms such as fearful, aggressive and docile (for more details on this terminology refer to Carter et al., Reference Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw and Heinsohn2013). There is growing evidence that personality traits such as exploration, reactivity and sociability are associated with measures of growth and productivity in ruminants (Haskell et al., Reference Greenwood, Hunt, Hermanson and Bell2014). Individual variability in feeding behaviour, and the way in which individuals respond to their feeding environment may also be related to the personality of the individual.

The aim of this review is to critically examine the available literature describing individual variability in feeding behaviour of ruminants, and how this variability relates to personality traits in animals. We will focus especially on ‘exploratory’ and ‘sociability’ traits that have received limited attention compared with the more commonly cited ‘fear’ (Forkman et al., Reference Favati, Leimar, Løvlie, Weissing and Winberg2007) and ‘reactivity’ traits (Haskell et al., Reference Greenwood, Hunt, Hermanson and Bell2014). We first describe how variability in development and expression of feeding behaviour may contribute to differences in growth and productivity, and then describe how personality traits may play a role in how individuals interact with and respond to challenges faced in the feeding environment. Throughout we review evidence in both grazing (e.g. extensively raised on rangeland or intensively raised on pasture) and confined (e.g. raised indoors or finished on feedlots) systems.

Individual variability in feeding behaviour

Development of feeding behaviour

The young ruminant relies initially on milk and begins sampling solid feed within the first few weeks after birth (Nicol and Sharafeldin, Reference Nicol1975). The timing of the transition from nursing to a solid diet is highly variable among individuals; for example, natural weaning in domestic cattle was reported to be between 7 and 14 months after birth (Reinhardt and Reinhardt, Reference Rice, Jongman, Borg, Butler and Hemsworth1981). Young ruminants will begin to graze by learning from social models such as the mother and conspecifics or learning by trial and error, leading to individual preferences and aversions to plants, and individual differences in ability to forage efficiently (Provenza and Balph, Reference Provenza and Burritt1987; Kyriazakis et al., Reference Kurvers, Hamblin and Giraldeau1999). Even at an older age, social models may be useful when introducing naïve animals to new feeding systems; when dairy heifers were turned out to pasture for the first time without an experienced companion, some individuals took over 3 h to begin to graze compared with just 1 h for those that were pastured with an experienced grazer (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Adderley, Weary and von Keyserlingk2016a).

In most dairy cattle production systems, calves are raised apart from their mother. Dairy farms vary in how much and how often milk is delivered to the calves and in opportunities for social learning (Vasseur et al., Reference Veissier and Stefanova2010; Hötzel et al., Reference Hewitson, Gordon and Dumont2014; USDA, Reference Val-Laillet, Passillé, Rushen and von Keyserlingk2014); both factors may influence the development of feeding behaviours (reviewed by Miller-Cushon and DeVries, Reference Miller-Cushon and DeVries2015). For example, calves reared individually must learn on their own where, how and what to eat. The lack of a social model may be particularly important during the transition from milk onto a solid diet, especially given that weaning occurs much earlier than in nature (Enríquez et al., Reference Dumont, Boissy, Achard, Sibbald and Erhard2011; reviewed by Khan et al., Reference Jørgensen, Andersen and Bøe2016).

Young ruminants vary in the amount of milk that they choose to consume when milk is provided ad libitum. This variability can contribute to differences in growth rates during the pre-weaning period. For example, de Passillé et al. (Reference de Passillé and Rushen2016) reported a large range in milk intakes during the first 2 to 4 days of age in Holstein dairy calves, ranging from 2.4 l to 12 l/day (7 to 30% of BW), resulting in differences in BW gains (ranging from 0.07 to 1.2 kg/day in the first month of age). Similar variability in milk feeding patterns were reported for artificially-reared lambs, ranging from 0.3 to 2.9 l/day milk consumption (David et al., Reference David, Bouvier, Ricard, Ruesche and Weisbecker2014).

Dairy calves raised indoors are typically introduced to a concentrate diet soon after birth. Considerable variability in concentrate consumption has been reported by several authors. Neave et al. (2018) showed that calves first found and began to consume grain at between 4 to 41 days of age when fed either 6, 8, 10 or 12 l/day of milk. Calves fed 12 l/day of milk first consumed 200 g/day of grain at between 23 to 82 days of age and first reached a daily grain consumption of 1400 g/day at between 58 to 94 days of age (de Passillé and Rushen, Reference Patison, Swain, Bishop-Hurley, Pattison and Robins2016). Calves fed less milk typically begin to consume more grain at an earlier age; however, Roth et al. (Reference Rushen, Taylor and de Passillé2009) showed that even when calves were fed just 6 l of milk/day, the age range when they first consumed 2000 g/day of grain was between 45 and 98 days of age. Furthermore, when group-housed calves were offered free choice of milk replacer, concentrate, maize silage, hay and straw, there was large individual variability in intake of each component, suggesting that calves develop diet preferences from a young age (Webb et al., Reference Webb, Engel, Berends, van Reenen, Gerrits, de Boer and Bokkers2014). These preferences may also arise from associations between sensory properties and nutritional value of the diet (Forbes and Kyriazakis, Reference Escós, Alados and Boza1995), the experiences associated with the first encounter with the feedstuff, or the physical properties of the diet that are important for stimulating ruminal development (Baumont, Reference Baumont1996).

This evidence highlights the variation in feeding behaviour among individuals from a young age. It is well known that reduced milk intake or reluctance to transition to solid feed can result in impaired growth during the pre-weaning and weaning periods in young ruminants (e.g. dairy calves: de Passillé et al., Reference de Passillé and Rushen2016; goat kids: Warmington and Kirton, Reference Warmington and Kirton1990; lambs: Greenwood et al., Reference Giraldeau and Caraco1998). Furthermore, early-life nutritional disadvantages have been shown to affect future foraging behaviour in other species (e.g. Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Viviani, Egan, Bedford, Brilot, Nettle and Bateson2015). Understanding how and why some individuals develop feeding patterns that result in better or worse performance is important if we are to help all animals thrive.

Expression of feeding behaviour

Characteristic feeding patterns that develop from an early age in ruminants appear to also be present in adulthood. The rearing environment, as well as morphological and physiological differences, will have a profound impact on how individuals express their feeding preferences and patterns as adults (Provenza and Balph, Reference Provenza and Burritt1987). Individuals within a herd can show feeding patterns that are widely variable between individuals but remain relatively consistent over time within individuals (Melin et al., Reference Meagher, Weary and von Keyserlingk2005). This is not to say that feeding behaviour is inflexible, but rather that the degree of flexibility in feeding patterns generally remains consistent within individuals over time.

The selection of, and preference for, plants to graze or browse will depend on the individual’s nutritional needs and on prior experience with these food sources and ability to cope with toxins (Provenza et al., Reference Ralphs, Graham and James2003). Ruminants are known to make trade-offs in selecting diets that meet the requirements of their internal state (e.g. hunger, and physiological state such as pregnancy) while reducing costs in the selection of the diet (e.g. environmental or social pressures) (Kyriazakis et al., Reference Kurvers, Hamblin and Giraldeau1999; Arsenos et al., Reference Arsenos, Emmans and Kyriazakis2000). Thus an individual’s diet selection is flexible with changing internal state, with changes in diet dependent upon any nutritional deficiency and post-ingestive feedback from ingested foods (e.g. Tolkamp et al., 1998). Day et al. (Reference Day, Kyriazakis and Rogers1998) proposed that food selection is also influenced by a motivation to explore the feeding environment, which functions to identify new food items and to monitor and update information on existing food sources; individuals are able to modify their feeding behaviour if needed.

Foraging animals must choose between continuing to exploit an existing site or searching for a superior foraging site (see Giraldeau and Caraco, Reference Galindo and Broom2000). One approach to understanding this dichotomy is the producer-scrounger model, originally developed to describe the feeding strategies of sparrows (Barnard and Sibly, Reference Barnard and Sibly1981). ‘Producers’ take the role of finding higher food quality food patches. These individuals benefit from first access to the new patch, but pay the cost of lost foraging opportunities (and perhaps increased predation) while searching for new patches (Giraldeau and Caraco, Reference Galindo and Broom2000). Other individuals adopt a ‘scrounger’ strategy of following ‘producers’ to exploit their findings rather than searching for food themselves. This framework has been applied to foraging strategies in goats (Stears et al., Reference Thouless2014) and sheep (Hewitson, Reference Haskell, Simm and Turner2002). Individuals within a herd may also adopt ‘leader’ and ‘follower’ roles in making decisions when to move between feeding locations including cattle (Dumont et al., Reference Dingemanse, Kazem, Réale and Wright2005), sheep (Squires and Daws, Reference Stears, Kerley and Shrader1975), goats (Escós et al., Reference Eitam, Vaya, Brosh, Orlov, Khatib, Izhaki and Shabtay1993) and buffalo (Prins, Reference Proudfoot and Habing1996). ’Producers’ may be more likely to be ‘leaders’ but this line of research has yet to be explored.

Given the evidence of individual variability in diet selection of foraging animals (Kyriazakis et al., Reference Kurvers, Hamblin and Giraldeau1999; Arsenos et al., Reference Arsenos, Emmans and Kyriazakis2000), it follows that there would be similar variability in the feeding patterns of confined ruminants. For instance, Melin et al. (Reference Meagher, Weary and von Keyserlingk2005) found that as much as 84% to 98% of the variation in feeding patterns could be attributed to individual differences between dairy cows. Several studies have shown substantial between-cow variability for meal frequency (e.g. ranging from 5 to 9 meals/day) and feeding time (dairy cattle: 250 to 450 min/day; beef cattle: 8 to 90 min/day or 86 to 120 min/day) (Schwartzkopf-Genswein et al., Reference Sibbald, Erhard, Hooper, Dumont and Boissy2002; DeVries et al., Reference de Passillé, Rabeyrin and Rushen2003). Such feeding patterns have been attributed more to phenotypic than to genetic variation among individuals (Løvendahl and Munksgaard, Reference Loerch and Fluharty2016).

Studies have also shown that some individuals will adjust their feeding behaviour in response to social or environmental changes. Crossley et al. (Reference Crossley, Harlander-Matauschek and DeVries2017) reported that when dairy cows competed for access to a feeding area, there was an increase in variability in feeding time, feeding rate and meal duration. In a companion study, increasing feeding frequency did not reduce variability in feeding time, feeding rate or dry matter intake (Crossley et al., Reference Crossley, Harlander-Matauschek and DeVries2018). This variability is likely due to individual motivations to access the feed bunk; some animals reduce feeding time and others strive to maintain feeding times even under high levels of competition (dairy cattle: Val-Laillet et al., Reference Van Reenen, O’Connell, Van Der Werf, Korte, Hopster, Jones and Blokhuis2008b; goats: Jørgensen et al., Reference Huzzey, DeVries, Valois and von Keyserlingk2007). Some individuals will also respond more negatively than others when experiencing environmental changes. For instance, Rice et al. (Reference Rice, Jongman, Butler and Hemsworth2016a) found that 18% of lambs entering a feedlot spent less than 30 min/day feeding, lost weight during the 1st week, and were more likely to visit the feeder when no other lambs were present. This evidence suggests that the feeding behaviour of some individuals will change in response to social or environmental pressure.

In summary, there is wide individual variability in feeding behaviour from an early age. Understanding why feeding behaviour is variable among individuals will be the focus of the remainder of this review, particularly how personality traits such as exploration, fear or reactivity and sociability affect how ruminants interact with their feeding environment.

Exploring the feeding environment

Foraging strategies

Studies on the personality characteristics of individuals adopting producer-scrounger or leader-follower foraging strategies often profile animals along an exploration-avoidance or boldness-shyness axis, and individuals that are more exploratory or bold are thought to be more likely be leaders or producers by searching for food rather than relying on others (Kurvers et al., Reference Krohn and Konggaard2012). For example, sheep that were more exploratory in an unfamiliar arena with novel objects were also more likely to move away from conspecifics while grazing, enabling them to explore more of the pasture area (Sibbald et al., Reference Sibbald and Hooper2009). Sheep that were more exploratory were also more likely to split into smaller subgroups, indicating these animals made the trade-off to explore their feeding environment rather than to remain together as a cohesive group (Michelena et al., Reference Melin, Wiktorsson and Norell2009). Domestic deer that spent more time close to or investigating novel objects made a similar trade-off, spending less time engaged in vigilant behaviours and more time investigating a novel food (Bergvall et al., Reference Bergvall, Schäpers, Kjellander and Weiss2011). In addition, beef heifers that spent more time interacting with a novel object tended to be positioned at the front of the herd (Ramseyer et al., Reference Rault, Lay and Marchant-Forde2009). These studies suggest that more exploratory individuals (as indicated by greater investigation of novel objects or food) adopt riskier foraging behaviour and that this results in increased opportunities to forage.

In confined farming systems, ruminants are typically provided uniform diets at specific times of the day, reducing the need for deciding when and where to forage. Indoor-housed animals will still perform exploratory behaviour, particularly when the feed quality is variable (Huzzey et al., Reference Hötzel, Longo, Balcão, Cardoso and Costa2013). Meagher et al. (Reference Lynch, Hinch and Adams2017) offered feed bins with different forage varieties or flavours along a feed bunk and recorded the number of bin switches as a measure of exploratory feed sampling. Heifers that spent more time in contact with a novel object in a previous test also spent more time exploring and eating the varied feed. Moreover, those that were quicker to reach a novel food in the individual test also spent more time at the varied feed and switched between bins more often. Most recently, dairy calves that were most exploratory in a novel environment began to eat grain earlier, consumed more grain and gained more weight (Neave et al., Reference Nawroth, von Borell and Langbein2018). Collectively this evidence indicates that some individuals are more proficient in exploring and sampling varied or novel feeds.

Feed sampling and sorting behaviour

Dairy cows are known to preferentially sort for concentrate and against long forage components in a mixed ration but this type of behaviour is highly variable among individuals, with some even sorting against the typically preferred finer particles (Leonardi and Armentano, Reference Lensink, Fernandez, Boivin, Pradel, Le Neindre and Veissier2003). Interestingly, sorting behaviour did not decrease when cows were fed in a competitive feeding environment (Hosseinkhani et al., Reference Hewitson2008), suggesting that individuals engaging in this behaviour are motivated to do so even when access to feed is limited. A possible explanation for this finding is that individuals that continued to sort in a competitive environment were also higher in social rank and thus could maintain their position at the feed bunk (see Favati et al., Reference Enríquez, Hötzel and Ungerfeld2014).

Sorting is also likely a learned behaviour, related to post-ingestive feedback mechanisms (Provenza, Reference Provenza and Balph1995), and familiarity of feed from an early age (Miller-Cushon and DeVries, Reference Michelena, Sibbald, Erhard and McLeod2011). Consequently, young dairy calves are able to sort a mixed ration and will adjust this behaviour in response to the availability of grain (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Costa, Weary, Machado Filho and von Keyserlingk2016b). Feed sorting is seen as a risk factor for ruminal acidosis in adult cows (Cook et al., Reference Cook, Nordlund and Oetzel2004), so farms often strive to prevent this type of behaviour. If indeed the motivation behind sorting behaviour stems from a desire to explore the feeding environment, management practices may be able to redirect this behaviour by offering other opportunities for environmental exploration or manipulation. To our knowledge, no work to date has explored such opportunities.

Overall, individuals differ in how much they explore their feeding environment. Some individuals are producers or scroungers, and some are leaders or followers when it comes to deciding how and where to find food. In confined housing, these foraging differences have yet to be documented, but individuals that sample their feeding environment have been shown to be more exploratory and bold in novel situations. Individuals that are more reactive in response to novelty (rather than exploratory or bold) also show differences in their feeding behaviour; this will be the focus of the next section.

Coping with stress: response patterns and fearfulness

Domestic ruminants experience many stressors; differences in the behavioural responses of individuals toward these stressors are called ‘coping styles’ (Benus et al., Reference Benus, Bohus, Koolhaas and van Oortmerssen1991; Koolhaas et al., Reference Koolhaas, De Boer, Buwalda and Van Reenen1999). Individuals also differ in their level of fear when responding to a stressful event, where more fearful individuals typically show stronger behavioural responses (e.g. Boissy, Reference Boissy1995). The quality (i.e. coping style) and magnitude or quantity of the response (i.e. fearfulness) may reflect two independent dimensions that together describe how individuals respond to a particular stressor (Koolhaas et al., Reference Khan, Bach, Weary and von Keyserlingk2007). This framework is supported by the multivariate analysis of the behavioural responses of dairy heifers exposed to different stressors including a novel environment, stationary human, novel object and restraint (Van Reenen et al., Reference Vasseur, Borderas, Cue, Lefebvre, Pellerin, Rushen, Wade and de Passillé2002, Reference Van Reenen, Van der Werf, Bruckmaier, Hopster, Engel, Noordhuizen and Blokhuis2005).

For some individuals, adjustments to their feeding behaviour may be a coping mechanism in response to changes in the feeding environment or other stressors. There is growing evidence in ruminants that feeding behaviour, including feed intake, may be reduced when individuals that are particularly fearful experience stressful events such as changes in the environment or feed type, or handling and restraint by humans. We will review this evidence in the next sections.

Reactivity to novel environments

Dairy heifers are known to differ in their responses when first introduced to the milking parlour; Van Reenen et al. (Reference Vasseur, Borderas, Cue, Lefebvre, Pellerin, Rushen, Wade and de Passillé2002) showed that some individuals had consistently higher physiological (i.e. cortisol) and behavioural reactivity (i.e. stepping and kicking) during milking preparation and teat cup attachment. Heifers habituated to the milking parlour for several weeks before calving had improved feed intake compared with those that were not habituated (Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Donkin, Eicher, Pajor and Schutz2007). Thus changes in feeding behaviour may be an indication of individuals that are especially affected by novel processes like milking.

Several authors have reported individual variability in physiological stress responses in cattle (e.g. Loerch and Fluharty, Reference Llonch, Somarriba, Duthie, Haskell, Rooke, Troy, Roehe and Turner1999; Eitam et al., Reference Dumont and Boissy2010) and lambs (Rice et al., Reference Rioja-Lang, Roberts, Healy, Lawrence and Haskell2016b) when introduced to a feedlot. Lambs with a high cortisol response during the 1st week in this new environment were also more reactive during isolation and restraint tests and had a greater number of feeding bouts likely due to a high number of displacements.

Some personality traits may drive consistent behavioural responses across a range of situations, whereas other traits may only manifest under specific contexts (Sloan Wilson et al., Reference Squires and Daws1994; Beausoleil et al., Reference Beausoleil, Blache, Stafford, Mellor and Noble2012). For instance, lambs that spent < 30 min/day feeding in the 1st week after arriving at the feedlot (termed ‘shy-feeders’) had no relationship with behavioural responses during isolation and restraint (Rice et al., Reference Rice, Jongman, Butler and Hemsworth2016a). However, this study used a standardized test that elicited a fear response specific to isolation which may be unrelated to responses to stressors encountered in a highly social feeding environment. Future work should aim to identify personality traits that are specific to individuals that show changes in their feeding behaviour when introduced to new environments and how modifications to that environment may be beneficial.

Food neophobia

Food neophobia, defined as a reluctance to eat unfamiliar foods (Chapple and Lynch, Reference Chapple and Lynch1986), is well-known in ruminants and is thought to help animals avoid toxic plants (Provenza and Balph, Reference Provenza and Burritt1987). This fear of novel diets must be overcome for livestock to transition to different feed types (Launchbaugh et al., Reference Launchbaugh and Howery1997). When this transition is coupled with a change in environment, food neophobia is greater compared with when animals remain in familiar environments (Burritt and Provenza, Reference Burritt and Provenza1997). Individuals will even consume familiar foods containing toxins over novel feeds when in an unfamiliar environment (Burritt and Provenza, Reference Burritt and Provenza1997). However, early exposure to a diversity of foods can increase acceptance of novel foods especially in unfamiliar locations (Villalba et al., Reference Villalba, Manteca and Provenza2012).

Variability in feeding behaviour may be due in part to differences in food neophobia. For instance, Rice et al. (Reference Rice, Jongman, Butler and Hemsworth2016a) suggested that highly reduced feeding times in some lambs (‘shy-feeders’) may be due to individual differences in food neophobia. This reluctance to sample novel feeds can be consistent over time and in different contexts. For example, Costa et al. (Reference Costa, von Keyserlingk and Weary2014) demonstrated that dairy calves were consistent across days in their willingness to sample two types of novel foods (carrots and hay), and heifers that were quick to find and eat more of a novel food in an arena also spent more time eating flavoured and varied forages offered at the feed bunk (Meagher et al., Reference Lynch, Hinch and Adams2017). In lambs, individuals that were more food neophobic were also more fearful in a novel arena and exhibited more stress-induced hyperthermia (Villalba et al., Reference von Keyserlingk and Weary2009), suggesting that the test of food neophobia reflects fearfulness. These experimental findings were recently supported in a study of pair-housed calves on a commercial farm that consumed nearly three times as much novel food compared with isolated calves (Whalin et al., Reference Whalin, Weary and von Keyserlingk2018). No studies have investigated how food neophobia is related to other personality traits, such as exploration and sociability.

Reactivity to handling

Individuals differ in their reactivity to handling and restraint. Many such interactions occur in farm animal production including vaccinations, dehorning, branding or castrating. With the advances of automated management technologies, opportunities for positive interactions and habituation to humans may be limited (Rushen et al., Reference Rutter1999; Butler et al., Reference Butler, Holloway and Bear2012). Poor handling and fear of humans are expected to alter the behaviour of the animals, including changes in feeding behaviour. For instance, beef cattle that are especially reactive when in the chute or isolated in a pen with a handler (i.e. nervous, vigorous or violent movement, or attempts to escape), and have high flight speeds exiting the chute, also have reduced feed intake (Black et al., 2013; Llonch et al., 2018), reduced feeding times (Cafe et al., 2011) and increased feeding bouts of shorter duration (Llonch et al., 2018). Similar effects on feeding behaviour have been shown in physiologically more reactive cattle (e.g. high cortisol response when in the chute; Llonch et al., Reference Leonardi and Armentano2016). However, other studies have not found a relationship between reactivity and feed intake (Petherick et al., Reference Prins2002; Francisco et al., Reference Forbes and Kyriazakis2015) or feeding time (Nkrumah et al., Reference Nicol and Sharafeldin2007). These inconsistent results may be related to how personality measures were analysed (e.g. categorical, such as ‘adequate’ v. ‘excitable’ in Francisco et al., Reference Forbes and Kyriazakis2015, or continuous, such as scoring personality from 1 to 5 in Cafe et al., Reference Cafe, Robinson, Ferguson, Mcintyre, Geesink and Greenwood2011) or may be related to the degree of negative experiences associated with humans (e.g. blood samples were taken before personality measures were scored in the chute; Cafe et al., Reference Cafe, Robinson, Ferguson, Mcintyre, Geesink and Greenwood2011).

Apart from feed intake and feeding time, no studies have examined how reactivity to handling influences other aspects of feeding behaviour in ruminants. However, work on other farm animals shows an association. For example, reactivity during weighing was associated with more visits to the feeder and less intake per visit in pigs (Ros-Freixedes et al., Reference Roth, Keil, Gygax and Hillmann2014). Rohrer et al. (Reference Ros-Freixedes, Sadler, Onteru, Smith, Young, Johnson, Lonergan, Huff-Lonergan, Dekkers and Rothschild2013) found that pigs that struggled more during restraint in the supine position (i.e. ‘reactive’ pigs) tended to have fewer daily meals and these were of longer duration compared with ‘proactive’ pigs. ‘Reactive’ pigs also preferred to eat at times when the feeder was less occupied. These studies suggest that active movement during restraint may be related to active avoidance of social conflict at the feeder.

Stress from prolonged or repeated unpleasant handling can lead to impaired growth and productivity (e.g. Lensink et al., Reference Launchbaugh, Provenza and Werkmeister2000). Whether growth is reduced due to changes in feeding behaviour that limit feed intake, or due to poorer feed efficiency, remains unknown. Recent research has shown that human presence can have a profound impact on goat behaviour; even changes in a human’s head position can alter the behaviour of goats (Nawroth et al., Reference Miranda-de la Lama, Sepúlveda, Montaldo, María and Galindo2015). Thus, the behaviour of stockpersons during management practices may elicit stress responses that in turn affect feeding behaviour. Together this evidence suggests that reactive individuals may be more prone to changes in feeding behaviour.

In summary, individuals differ in the way they respond to stressful events on farms. Feeding behaviour and feed intake can be impaired in individuals that are particularly reactive to a change in environment (feedlot, milking parlour), change in diet (food neophobia) and handling by humans (e.g. restraint in a chute). Aspects of the social environment may also be stressful for some individuals, and the way in which individuals respond to stressors may be related to the social relationships within the herd. We turn to this topic in the following section.

Social relationships

Most farm animals are housed in groups, resulting in a feeding environment that also involves interactions with other individuals. This can be a source of stress for some individuals, especially when the number of animals exceeds resource availability and results in competition for food (Proudfoot and Habing, Reference Provenza2015). The social context, including social relationships among individuals, has a major effect on behaviour. These social relationships can be agonistic (e.g. dominant-subordinate) or affiliative (e.g. sociability).

Dominant-subordinate relationships

Domestic ruminants are gregarious and will organize themselves into social hierarchies with dominant and subordinate individuals (e.g. cattle: Bouissou et al., Reference Bouissou, Boissy, Le Neindre and Veissier2001; goats: Miranda-de la Lama and Mattiello, Reference Miller and Wood-Gush2010). An individual’s position in the hierarchy is often expressed through agonistic interactions when gaining or maintaining access to resources (Miller and Wood-Gush, Reference Miller-Cushon and DeVries1991; Barroso et al., Reference Barroso, Alados and Boza2000). For grazing ruminants on rangeland, where space is less limited, this hierarchy is related to priority of access to high-quality grazing areas (Barroso et al., Reference Barroso, Alados and Boza2000), which can be expressed as an ‘avoidance order’ whereby, subordinate animals avoid conflict with dominant ones (Sárová et al., Reference Schwartzkopf-Genswein, Atwood and McAllister2010).

Feeding behaviour is related to the social rank of the individual with dominant individuals typically having priority access to food. This is especially evident when forage is limited and low in quality. For example, higher-ranking goats will out-compete subordinates to consume preferred shrubs (Barroso et al., Reference Barroso, Alados and Boza2000) and dominant individuals are often more efficient foragers (e.g. Thouless, Reference Tolkamp, Kyriazakis, Oldham, Lewis, Dewhurst and Newbold1990), likely due to less selection required when given priority access to feed resources. To achieve these foraging advantages, dominant animals position themselves toward the front of the herd and cover shorter distances relative to subordinates during periods of foraging (Sárová et al., Reference Schwartzkopf-Genswein, Atwood and McAllister2010). Consequently, subordinates are often forced to graze areas of lower quality; these individuals could move away from the group in search of improved grazing opportunity but risk increased exposure to predators (Thouless, Reference Tolkamp, Kyriazakis, Oldham, Lewis, Dewhurst and Newbold1990; Barroso et al., Reference Barroso, Alados and Boza2000). Indeed, the slower bite rate of subordinates is thought to reflect the trade-off between grazing and maintaining vigilance for predators and dominant animals, in addition to the increased necessity to select forage (Thouless, Reference Tolkamp, Kyriazakis, Oldham, Lewis, Dewhurst and Newbold1990). Interestingly, subordinates also reduce bite rate when dominant individuals are nearby, and cease grazing altogether to avoid neighbouring dominant animals (Thouless, Reference Tolkamp, Kyriazakis, Oldham, Lewis, Dewhurst and Newbold1990). Together this evidence indicates that dominant-subordinate relationships are important drivers of foraging behaviour in grazing systems.

Dominant–subordinate relationships in confined housing systems also influence feeding behaviour. Subordinate individuals may fail to gain access to the feed bunk and eat at times that are less preferred (Huzzey et al., Reference Hosseinkhani, DeVries, Proudfoot, Valizadeh, Veira and von Keyserlingk2006), and even sacrifice higher quality feed to avoid feeding near a dominant (Rioja-Lang et al., Reference Rohrer, Brown-Brandl, Rempel, Schneider and Holl2009). These effects may be exacerbated when competition for feed increases (e.g. Jørgensen et al., Reference Huzzey, DeVries, Valois and von Keyserlingk2007). Although dominance rank is often scored as the number and outcome of agonistic interactions between dyads (Galindo and Broom, Reference Francisco, Resende, Benatti, Castilhos, Cooke and Jorge2000), displacements at the feeder by cattle has been reported to be bi- or tri-directional and nonlinear with subordinate cows occasionally displacing dominants (Val-Laillet et al., Reference Val-Laillet, Veira and von Keyserlingk2008a). Of most interest is that high-ranking cows do not necessarily have the longest feeding times; dominance at the feed bunk may be related to individual motivation to gain access to feed or to defending the resource (Val-Laillet et al., Reference Val-Laillet, Veira and von Keyserlingk2008a; Reference Van Reenen, O’Connell, Van Der Werf, Korte, Hopster, Jones and Blokhuisb).

Variability in social behaviour at the feeder cannot be explained entirely by dominance. For example, Miranda-de la Lama et al. (Reference Miranda-de la Lama and Mattiello2011) described four social strategies or ‘identity profiles’ in goats: ‘passive’ goats (submissive but made no attempt to avoid or engage in agonistic behaviour) spent the least time at the feeder, whereas ‘avoider’ goats (submissive and avoided both agonistic and even non-agonistic behaviour) spent the most time feeding. Goats that were ‘aggressive’ (highly dominant and mediated other social conflicts) and ‘affiliative’ (average dominance and engaged in socio-positive behaviours) were similar in time spent at the feeder but intermediate to ‘avoider’ and ‘passive’ goats. Thus individuals can share similar dominance ranks yet adopt different social strategies that impact feeding behaviour.

Temporal feeding patterns and other measures of feeding behaviour such as feeding rate can further our understanding of how individuals are able to maintain or adjust feeding time under competitive conditions when adopting different social strategies. Zobel et al. (Reference Zobel, Schwartzkopf-Genswein, Genswein and von Keyserlingk2011) noted that beef heifers varied in how they responded to a highly competitive feeding environment; some individuals actively engaged in competition while maintaining a relatively stable feeding rate, but others appeared to actively avoid competition and shifted their feeding until after peak feeding time. Still, others appeared to adopt a ‘get-in-and-get-out’ strategy characterized by increased feeding rate and avoidance of agonistic interactions. Indeed, Nielsen (1999) noted that changes in feeding rate could be a valuable indicator of social pressure experienced by individuals in group feeding environments.

These results suggest avenues for future research. Given that individuals differ in their responsiveness to environmental change (i.e. behavioural plasticity; reviewed in Dingemanse et al., Reference de Passillé and Rushen2010), we might expect that some individuals will adjust their social behaviour at the feed bunk in response to a change in social environment (e.g. reduce aggression when there is low stocking density or when there are many other dominant individuals), whereas others will remain consistent across different social environments (e.g. maintain aggression despite plenty of space to access feed). This research would help to advance our understanding of, and opportunities for management of, the social grouping of ruminants in both grazing and confined feeding environments.

Affiliative relationships and sociability

Herd-living animals also have affiliative relationships, and these are also likely to influence social behaviour in the feeding environment. The formation of affiliative relationships among individuals has been reported among domestic ruminants, including cattle (Bouissou et al., Reference Bouissou, Boissy, Le Neindre and Veissier2001), goats (Miranda-de la Lama and Mattiello, Reference Miller and Wood-Gush2010) and sheep (Lynch et al., Reference Løvendahl and Munksgaard1992). Dairy cows form and maintain preferred partnerships from an early age (Raussi et al., Reference Reinhardt and Reinhardt2010) with large within- and between-cow variability in the frequency of social interactions and time spent in close proximity to partners (Gutmann et al., Reference Goetsch, Gipson, Askar and Puchala2015). This variability in sociability has been proposed as a distinct personality trait underlying how individuals respond to environmental challenges (Koolhaas and Van Reenen, Reference Koolhaas, Korte, De Boer, Van Der Vegt, Van Reenen, Hopster, De Jong, Ruis and Blokhuis2016). Sociability can be defined as the motivation to remain close to conspecifics (Sibbald et al., Reference Sibbald, Erhard, McLeod and Hooper2005), and appears to be linked with feeding patterns of grazing animals that must make trade-offs between social and feeding motivations. For example, when high-quality forage is distributed in patches, the intake and diet composition of each individual depends on their willingness to move away from the herd to graze preferred patches (Sibbald and Hooper, Reference Sibbald and Hooper2003). In a subsequent paper, these authors (Sibbald and Hooper, Reference Sih and Bell2004) demonstrated that more sociable sheep (i.e. those that spent the most time in close proximity to other sheep) were less likely to move away from the group to access a preferred grazing patch.

These foraging trade-offs appear to be influenced by the strength of relationships with herd mates. Dumont and Boissy (Reference DeVries, von Keyserlingk, Weary and Beauchemin2000) reported that ewes penned with familiar companions chose to graze away from the group for longer periods, vocalized less and were less vigilant than those grazing with unfamiliar companions. Brahman steers were also more willing to leave a familiar companion to approach a food bowl but did not do so when housed with an unfamiliar companion (Patison et al., Reference Petherick, Holroyd, Doogan and Venus2010). These authors suggested that a combination of a lack of social support and fear of isolation motivated individuals to remain close to unfamiliar companions. Sociability is also related to group movements between foraging sites. Ramseyer et al. (Reference Rault, Lay and Marchant-Forde2009) demonstrated that heifers with limited affiliative partnerships and ewes that often grazed away from the group were those that most often initiated group movement.

In confined housing systems there is also evidence that individual differences in sociability influence feeding behaviour. Gibbons et al. (Reference Galef and Laland2010) found that dairy cows who took longer to return to the group following separation at the end of a passageway had more limited partner associations, were less synchronized with the group, and did not feed during peak feeding times. The strength and type of partner associations appears to affect feeding behaviour. Individuals housed together for longer periods were more likely to be preferential partners during feeding (dairy cattle: Gutmann et al., Reference Goetsch, Gipson, Askar and Puchala2015; goats: Aschwanden et al., Reference Aschwanden, Gygax, Wechsler and Keil2008), and these feeding partners showed more positive social contact such as allogrooming (Val-Laillet et al., Reference Val-Laillet, Veira and von Keyserlingk2008a). These studies suggest that preferential associations among individuals can influence feeding behaviour in both grazing and confined herds. Future research should aim to understand how the quality and quantity of relationships between individuals influences how individuals make foraging decisions and affect feeding patterns. For instance, individuals that develop close social bonds may become more reliant on a social partner to find high-quality food in a grazing system. Furthermore, individuals with strong social bonds or many social partnerships may be better able to cope with stressful feeding environments given that social partners often mediate stress responses in farm animals (Rault et al., Reference Raussi, Niskanen, Siivonen, Hã, Hepola, Jauhiainen and Veissier2011).

Social facilitation and social learning

Social companions are important sources of information for making foraging decisions in both grazing and confined housing systems. Social facilitation and social learning play important roles in the development of foraging behaviour in neonatal ruminants on pasture (Launchbaugh and Howery, Reference Laland2005). Social facilitation is the phenomenon where the stimulus of another animal eating, approaching or manipulating feed may increase attention toward the feed, and subsequently encourage consumption of feed by others, whereas social learning describes the mechanism of learning through observation of others (Zentall and Galef, Reference Zentall and Galef1988).

Young ruminants learn from their mother and conspecifics the location of food, water and shelter, and consequently develop diet and habitat use patterns that resemble those of social models (Provenza and Balph, Reference Provenza and Burritt1987). For instance, individuals that were conditioned to avoid a particular plant when grazing alone began to consume this plant when grazing with others that had not learned to avoid this plant (cattle: Ralphs et al., Reference Ramseyer, Boissy, Thierry and Dumont1994; lambs: Provenza and Burritt, Reference Provenza, Villalba, Dziba, Atwood and Banner1991). Social models can be influential in learning how to graze; naïve beef calves had increased grazing activity when turned out to pasture with experienced companions compared with those without an experienced social model (Hessle, Reference Gutmann, Špinka and Winckler2009).

For young ruminants that are raised in confinement, social facilitation and social learning may influence how quickly individuals begin to use feeding equipment. Lambs were quicker to learn to drink milk from a teat when grouped with lambs that had previously learned this behaviour (Veissier and Stefanova, Reference Villalba, Catanese, Provenza and Distel1993). Dairy calves raised with a social partner consumed more starter during the milk feeding period (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Meagher, von Keyserlingk and Weary2015), were quicker to first visit a new concentrate feeder when regrouped after weaning (de Paula Vieira et al., 2010) and had more frequent concentrate meals before and during weaning (Miller-Cushon and DeVries, 2016) compared with calves raised alone. Consequently, socially housed calves have improved feed intake and weight gains (reviewed by Costa et al., Reference Costa, Daros, von Keyserlingk and Weary2016c). Together the evidence in both grazed and housed ruminant species suggests that learning about the feeding environment is influenced by the presence of social models.

Social foraging theory suggests that obtaining information by observing others is less costly than gaining the same information personally (Galef and Laland, Reference Forkman, Boissy, Meunier-Salaün, Canali and Jones2005). An exciting area of research is how individuals differ in their use of social information, such that some may be more likely to learn from social information in a feeding context. Sih and Bell (Reference Sloan Wilson, Clark, Coleman and Dearstyne2008) suggest that more sociable individuals may be more likely to acquire social information, given their close proximity and increased interactions among group mates compared with less sociable individuals. Indeed, the development and extent of social relationships among individuals in a grazing herd of cattle have been shown to be critical in the efficiency of information transfer (Launchbaugh and Howery, Reference Laland2005). Similarly, the development of solid feeding behaviour in young ruminants requiring the discovery and exploitation of a novel food resource may be influenced by the sociability of the animal such that strong affiliations may lead to increased attention to social cues. Naïve observers may pay more attention to experienced demonstrators if they share a particular relationship (see review by Nicol, Reference Neisen, Wechsler and Gygax1995).

The personality of the demonstrator may also suggest to observers within the herd that they are a reliable source of information. For instance, observers may watch and learn from the foraging behaviour of more exploratory or dominant individuals (Nicol, Reference Neisen, Wechsler and Gygax1995). Indeed, there is evidence suggesting that the feeding behaviour of dominant individuals is copied by subordinates (Laland, Reference Kyriazakis, Tolkamp and Emmans2004). This line of research has been tested in some ruminants. In sheep, dominance status of both the observer and demonstrator affected decisions to leave a food patch for a potentially better foraging opportunity (Hewitson et al., Reference Hessle2007); subordinate sheep would only follow a dominant when the expectation for discovering high-quality food was high. In contrast, Baciadonna et al. (Reference Baciadonna, McElligott and Briefer2013) found that the use of social information by goats to locate a food patch was not dependent upon the dominance rank of the demonstrator. However, these authors also found that goats favoured the use of personal rather than social information to locate food. Reliance upon personal information may be more important when foraging in a variable or patchy environment.

These studies illustrate how social status affects the use of information when making foraging decisions. However, no work in ruminants has explored the effects of affiliative relationships on the use of social information. In addition, efforts could be made to understand if some personality types utilize or convey social information more than others.

In summary, the social environment influences feeding behaviour in grazing and confined housing systems. Dominant–subordinate and affiliative relationships affect how individuals forage, gain access to feed and adjust feeding patterns when the social environment changes.

Implications for management and animal welfare

This review has highlighted the wide variability in feeding behaviour among individuals, and how personality differences can explain why some individuals struggle to learn about their feeding environment or fail to adapt to changes in their nutritional, social or physical environment. Our current understanding of the relationship between personality and feeding behaviour remains correlational; we encourage further research to determine causality of these associations. Nonetheless, there is considerable opportunity for farmers to tailor the management of their herd to better fit the needs of these individuals, and thus likely improve their welfare. Using the associations between personality traits and feeding behaviour that have emerged throughout this review we provide suggestions for how changes in management may improve feeding behaviour and intake in farmed ruminants.

In grazing herds, managers can take advantage of foraging strategies where individuals favour searching for new food sources themselves rather than relying upon others. ‘Producers’ are more likely to widen their grazing distribution and explore new habitats, whereas the more sociable individuals that tend to remain close to herd members are likely to follow the grazing patterns of the ‘producers’ (Launchbaugh and Howery, Reference Laland2005). Individuals with exploratory, bold or dominant personalities, or those with strong affiliative or familiar relationships, are known to be especially influential in directing the grazing patterns of the herd. Managers may be able to guide habitat use by cueing or training these individuals where to graze and subsequently may direct the grazing movements of the whole herd through information flow through the group.

In confined systems where the feed bunk is a highly social and often competitive environment, management of these social dynamics should be targeted. Farms that tend to overstock at the feed bunk, or have groups with many highly dominant or aggressive individuals, are likely to see more variable feeding patterns due to individuals adopting different social strategies to gain or maintain access to the feed bunk (e.g. Miranda-de la Lama et al., Reference Miranda-de la Lama and Mattiello2011; Zobel et al., Reference Zobel, Schwartzkopf-Genswein, Genswein and von Keyserlingk2011). Consequently, some individuals will be forced to increase their feeding rate, reduce feeding time or access the feed bunk at non-peak feeding times, potentially leading to limited or lower-quality feed intake. Managers should aim to provide ample space at the feed bunk and ensure pens have few highly dominant individuals that may monopolize access to the feed bunk; these strategies will help all individuals of the herd to express their preferred feeding patterns.

Farmed ruminants must adjust to stressors such as dietary and environmental transitions, including an introduction to unfamiliar environments like the feedlot or milking parlour, novel foods, or exposure to handling or restraint devices that often occur in conjunction with negative experiences. Negative responses to novelty that potentially impact feeding behaviour could be reduced by, for example, introducing novel diets before movement to a new feeding facility, or by pairing movement through a restraining device with a food reward. This is likely to be especially beneficial for individuals that are most reactive to stressful events. However, it is important to remember that all individuals require time to adapt to change.

Managers should also consider providing an experienced social model, especially when moving animals to new pens requiring the use of different feeding equipment (e.g. headlocks) or when introducing novel feeds (e.g. when artificially weaning young ruminants from milk onto a solid-feed diet). We suggest that individuals that are particularly calm in response to stressors may be effective social models for those that are more reactive, even if they are not knowledgeable or experienced; however, the social model must be familiar to the group to avoid an added stressor.

Some farms already allow for individuals to adapt to transition periods at their own pace. For instance, automated calf feeders have been used to wean calves individually when they reach specific grain consumption targets rather than applying one weaning program to all animals (e.g. de Passillé and Rushen, Reference Patison, Swain, Bishop-Hurley, Pattison and Robins2016). Heifers may also be kept in a separate lactating group after calving as they are often of lower social rank and subject to aggression when mixed with the main herd (Neisen et al., Reference Neave, Costa, Weary and von Keyserlingk2009). Managing these vulnerable individuals appropriately may improve access to the feed bunk and feeding time (Krohn and Konggaard, Reference Koolhaas and Van Reenen1979).

Conclusions

This review has illustrated the variability in feeding behaviour of domesticated ruminants and has argued that personality differences can explain why some individuals struggle to learn about, or fail to adapt, to changes in their feeding environment. The propensity of individuals to explore their feeding environment, the reactivity of individuals in response to common management stressors, dominance status and degree of sociability in the herd all affect the ability of individuals to access feed. With a better understanding of how personality influences feeding behaviour, individual management may improve the welfare of individuals, particularly those that have difficulty learning where and what to eat, are typically last to gain access to feed or expend more effort maintaining access to feed.

Acknowledgements

This review is based on an invited presentation given by MvK at the 10th International Symposium on the Nutrition of Herbivores held in Clermont-Ferrand, France, September 2018. The authors wish to thank Joao H.C. Costa (University of Kentucky) for his helpful input and discussion on topics relevant to this review. M.A.G.v.K. and D.M.W. are supported by Canada’s Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) via the Industrial Research Chair Program with industry contributions from the Dairy Farmers of Canada (Ottawa, ON, Canada), British Columbia Dairy Association (Burnaby, BC Canada), Westgen Endowment Fund (Milner, BC, Canada), Intervet Canada Corporation (Kirkland, QC, Canada), Zoetis (Kirkland, QC, Canada), Novus International Inc. (Oakville, ON, Canada), BC Cattle Industry Development Fund (Kamloops, BC, Canada), Alberta Milk (Edmonton, AB, Canada), Valacta (St. Anne-de-Bellevue, QC, Canada), and CanWest DHI (Guelph, ON, Canada).

Declaration of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethics statement

The present work involved a critical review of existing literature; our institution conducted no study requiring ethics approval specific to this review.

Software and data repository resources

None.

References

Andrews, C, Viviani, J, Egan, E, Bedford, T, Brilot, B, Nettle, D and Bateson, M 2015. Early life adversity increases foraging and information gathering in European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris . Animal Behaviour 109, 123132.Google Scholar
Arsenos, G, Emmans, GC and Kyriazakis, I 2000. Variation between individuals and the consequences for diet selection by groups of animals. Animal Behaviour 60, 811820.Google Scholar
Aschwanden, J, Gygax, L, Wechsler, B and Keil, NM 2008. Social distances of goats at the feeding rack: influence of the quality of social bonds, rank differences, grouping age and presence of horns. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 114, 116131.Google Scholar
Baciadonna, L, McElligott, AG and Briefer, EF 2013. Goats favour personal over social information in an experimental foraging task. PeerJ 1, e172.Google Scholar
Barnard, CJ and Sibly, RM 1981. Producers and scroungers: a general model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows. Animal Behaviour 29, 543550.Google Scholar
Barroso, FG, Alados, CL and Boza, J 2000. Social hierarchy in the domestic goat: effect on food habits and production. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 69, 3553.Google Scholar
Baumont, R 1996. Palatability and feeding behaviour in ruminants: a review. Annales de zootechnie 45, 385400.Google Scholar
Beausoleil, NJ, Blache, D, Stafford, KJ, Mellor, DJ and Noble, ADL 2012. Selection for temperament in sheep: domain-general and context-specific traits. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 139, 7485.Google Scholar
Benus, RF, Bohus, B, Koolhaas, JM and van Oortmerssen, GA 1991. Heritable variation for aggression as a reflection of individual coping strategies. Experientia 47, 10081019.Google Scholar
Bergvall, UA, Schäpers, A, Kjellander, P and Weiss, A 2011. Personality and foraging decisions in fallow deer, Dama dama. Animal Behaviour 81, 101112.Google Scholar
Black, TE, Bischoff, KM, Mercadante, VRG, Marquezini, GHL, DiLorenzo, N, Chase, CC, Coleman, SW, Maddock, TD and Lamb, GC 2013. Relationships among performance, residual feed intake, and temperament assessed in growing beef heifers and subsequently as 3-year-old, lactating beef cows. Journal of Animal Science 91, 22542263.Google Scholar
Boissy, A 1995. Fear and fearfulness in animals. The Quarterly Review of Biology 70, 165191.Google Scholar
Bouissou, M, Boissy, A, Le Neindre, P and Veissier, I 2001. The social behaviour of cattle. In Social behavior in farm animals (ed. J Keeling and H Gonyou), pp. 135147. CABI Publishing, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Burritt, EA and Provenza, FD 1997. Effect of an unfamiliar location on the consumption of novel and familiar foods by sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54, 317325.Google Scholar
Butler, D, Holloway, L and Bear, C 2012. The impact of technological change in dairy farming: robotic milking systems and the changing role of the stockperson. Journal of the Royal Agricultural Society of England 173, 16.Google Scholar
Cafe, LM, Robinson, DL, Ferguson, DM, Mcintyre, BL, Geesink, GH and Greenwood, PL 2011. Cattle temperament: persistence of assessments and associations with productivity, efficiency, carcass and meat quality traits. Journal of Animal Science 89, 14521465.Google Scholar
Carter, AJ, Feeney, WE, Marshall, HH, Cowlishaw, G and Heinsohn, R 2013. Animal personality: what are behavioural ecologists measuring? Biological Reviews 88, 465475.Google Scholar
Chapple, RS and Lynch, JJ 1986. Behavioural factors modifying acceptance of supplementary foods by sheep. Research and Development in Agriculture 3, 113120.Google Scholar
Cook, NB, Nordlund, KV and Oetzel, GR 2004. Environmental influences on claw horn lesions associated with laminitis and subacute ruminal acidosis in dairy cows. Journal of Dairy Science 87, E36E46.Google Scholar
Costa, JHC, Adderley, NA, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2016c. Short communication: effect of diet changes on sorting behavior of weaned dairy calves. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 56355639.Google Scholar
Costa, JHC, Costa, WG, Weary, DM, Machado Filho, LCP and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2016a. Dairy heifers benefit from the presence of an experienced companion when learning how to graze. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 562568.Google Scholar
Costa, JHC, Daros, RR, von Keyserlingk, MAG and Weary, DM 2014. Complex social housing reduces food neophobia in dairy calves. Journal of Dairy Science 97, 78047810.Google Scholar
Costa, JHC, Meagher, RK, von Keyserlingk, MAG and Weary, DM 2015. Early pair housing increases solid feed intake and weight gains in dairy calves. Journal of Dairy Science 98, 63816386.Google Scholar
Costa, JHC, von Keyserlingk, MAG and Weary, DM 2016b. Invited review: effects of group housing of dairy calves on behavior, cognition, performance, and health. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 24532467.Google Scholar
Crossley, RE, Harlander-Matauschek, A and DeVries, TJ 2017. Variability in behavior and production among dairy cows fed under differing levels of competition. Journal of Dairy Science 100, 38253838.Google Scholar
Crossley, R., Harlander-Matauschek, A and DeVries, T 2018. Mitigation of variability between competitively fed dairy cows through increased feed delivery frequency. Journal of Dairy Science 101, 518529.Google Scholar
Daniels, KJ, Donkin, SS, Eicher, SD, Pajor, EA and Schutz, MM 2007. Prepartum milking of heifers influences future production and health. Journal of Dairy Science 90, 22932301.Google Scholar
David, I, Bouvier, F, Ricard, E, Ruesche, J and Weisbecker, JL 2014. Feeding behaviour of artificially reared Romane lambs. Animal 8, 982990.Google Scholar
Day, JEL, Kyriazakis, I and Rogers, PJ 1998. Food choice and intake: towards a unifying framework of learning and feeding motivation. Nutrition Research Reviews 11, 2543.Google Scholar
de Passillé, AM, Rabeyrin, M and Rushen, J 2016. Associations between milk intake and activity in the first days of a calf’s life and later growth and health. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 175, 27.Google Scholar
de Passillé, AM and Rushen, J 2016. Using automated feeders to wean calves fed large amounts of milk according to their ability to eat solid feed. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 35783583.Google Scholar
de Paula Vieira A, von Keyserlingk MAG and Weary D 2010. Effects of pair versus single housing on performance and behavior of dairy calves before and after weaning from milk. Journal of Dairy Science 93, 3079–3085. Google Scholar
DeVries, TJ, von Keyserlingk, MAG, Weary, DM and Beauchemin, KA 2003. Measuring the feeding behavior of lactating dairy cows in early to peak lactation. Journal of Dairy Science 86, 33543361.Google Scholar
Dingemanse, NJ, Kazem, AJN, Réale, D and Wright, J 2010. Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality meets individual plasticity. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 25, 8189.Google Scholar
Dumont, B and Boissy, A 2000. Grazing behaviour of sheep in a situation of conflict between feeding and social motivations. Behavioural Processes 49, 131138.Google Scholar
Dumont, B, Boissy, A, Achard, C, Sibbald, AM and Erhard, HW 2005. Consistency of animal order in spontaneous group movements allows the measurement of leadership in a group of grazing heifers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 95, 5566.Google Scholar
Eitam, H, Vaya, J, Brosh, A, Orlov, A, Khatib, S, Izhaki, I and Shabtay, A 2010. Differential stress responses among newly received calves: variations in reductant capacity and Hsp gene expression. Cell Stress and Chaperones 15, 865876.Google Scholar
Enríquez, D, Hötzel, MJ and Ungerfeld, R 2011. Minimising the stress of weaning of beef calves: a review. Acta veterinaria Scandinavica 53, 28.Google Scholar
Escós, J, Alados, CL and Boza, J 1993. Leadership in a domestic goat herd. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38, 4147.Google Scholar
Favati, A, Leimar, O, Løvlie, H, Weissing, F and Winberg, S 2014. Personality predicts social dominance in male domestic fowl. PLoS ONE 9, e103535.Google Scholar
Forbes, JM and Kyriazakis, I 1995. Food preferences in farm animals: why don’t they always choose wisely? Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 54, 429440.Google Scholar
Forkman, B, Boissy, A, Meunier-Salaün, MC, Canali, E and Jones, RB 2007. A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses. Physiology and Behavior 92, 340374.Google Scholar
Francisco, CL, Resende, FD, Benatti, JMB, Castilhos, AM, Cooke, RF and Jorge, AM 2015. Impacts of temperament on Nellore cattle: physiological responses, feedlot performance, and carcass characteristics. Journal of Animal Science 93, 54195429.Google Scholar
Galef, BG and Laland, KN 2005. Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theoretical models. BioScience 55, 489.Google Scholar
Galindo, F and Broom, DM 2000. The relationships between social behaviour of dairy cows and the occurrence of lameness in three herds. Research in Veterinary Science 69, 7579.Google Scholar
Gibbons, JM, Lawrence, AB and Haskell, MJ 2010. Measuring sociability in dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 122, 8491.Google Scholar
Giraldeau, LA and Caraco, T 2000. Social foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, USA.Google Scholar
Goetsch, AL, Gipson, TA, Askar, AR and Puchala, R 2010. Invited review: feeding behavior of goats. Journal of Animal Science 88, 361373.Google Scholar
Greenwood, PL, Hunt, A., Hermanson, JW and Bell, A. 1998. Effects of birth weight and postnatal nutrition on neonatal sheep: I. Body growth and composition, and some aspects of energetic efficiency. Journal of Animal Science 76, 23542367.Google Scholar
Gutmann, AK, Špinka, M and Winckler, C 2015. Long-term familiarity creates preferred social partners in dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 169, 18.Google Scholar
Haskell, MJ, Simm, G and Turner, SP 2014. Genetic selection for temperament traits in dairy and beef cattle. Frontiers in Genetics 5, 368.Google Scholar
Hessle, AK 2009. Effects of social learning on foraging behaviour and live weight gain in first-season grazing calves. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 116, 150155.Google Scholar
Hewitson, L 2002. The foraging behaviour of sheep in response to environmental uncertainty. PhD thesis, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland.Google Scholar
Hewitson, L, Gordon, IJ and Dumont, B 2007. Social context affects patch-leaving decisions of sheep in a variable environment. Animal Behaviour 74, 239246.Google Scholar
Hosseinkhani, A, DeVries, TJ, Proudfoot, KL, Valizadeh, R, Veira, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2008. The effects of feed bunk competition on the feed sorting behavior of close-up dry cows. Journal of Dairy Science 91, 11151121.Google Scholar
Hötzel, MJ, Longo, C, Balcão, LF, Cardoso, CS and Costa, JHC 2014. A survey of management practices that influence performance and welfare of dairy calves reared in Southern Brazil. PLoS ONE 9, e114995.Google Scholar
Huzzey, JM, DeVries, TJ, Valois, P and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2006. Stocking density and feed barrier design affect the feeding and social behavior of dairy cattle. Journal of Dairy Science 89, 126133.Google Scholar
Huzzey, JM, Fregonesi, JA, von Keyserlingk, MAG and Weary, DM 2013. Sampling behavior of dairy cattle: effects of variation in dietary energy density on behavior at the feed bunk. Journal of Dairy Science 96, 247256.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, GHM, Andersen, IL and Bøe, KE 2007. Feed intake and social interactions in dairy goats – the effects of feeding space and type of roughage. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 107, 239251.Google Scholar
Khan, MA, Bach, A, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2016. Invited review: transitioning from milk to solid feed in dairy heifers. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 885902.Google Scholar
Koolhaas, JM, De Boer, SF, Buwalda, B and Van Reenen, K 2007. Individual variation in coping with stress: a multidimensional approach of ultimate and proximate mechanisms. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 70, 218226.Google Scholar
Koolhaas, JM, Korte, SM, De Boer, SF, Van Der Vegt, BJ, Van Reenen, CG, Hopster, H, De Jong, IC, Ruis, MAW and Blokhuis, HJ 1999. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23, 925935.Google Scholar
Koolhaas, JM and Van Reenen, CG 2016. Animal behavior and well-being symposium: interaction between coping style/personality, stress, and welfare: relevance for domestic farm animals. Journal of Animal Science 94, 22842296.Google Scholar
Krohn, CC and Konggaard, SP 1979. Effects of isolating first-lactation cows from older cows. Livestock Production Science 6, 137146.Google Scholar
Kurvers, RHJM, Hamblin, S and Giraldeau, LA 2012. The effect of exploration on the use of producer-scrounger tactics. PLoS ONE 7, e49400.Google Scholar
Kyriazakis, I, Tolkamp, BJ and Emmans, G 1999. Diet selection and animal state: an integrative framework. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 58, 765772.Google Scholar
Laland, KN 2004. Social learning strategies. Animal Learning & Behavior 32, 414.Google Scholar
Launchbaugh, KL and Howery, LD 2005. Understanding landscape use patterns of livestock as a consequence of foraging behavior. Rangeland Ecology & Management 58, 99108.Google Scholar
Launchbaugh, KL, Provenza, FD and Werkmeister, MJ 1997. Overcoming food neophobia in domestic ruminants through addition of a familiar flavor and repeated exposure to novel foods. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54, 327334.Google Scholar
Lensink, BJ, Fernandez, X, Boivin, X, Pradel, P, Le Neindre, P and Veissier, I 2000. The impact of gentle contacts on ease of handling, welfare, and growth of calves and on quality of veal meat. Journal of Animal Science 78, 12191226.Google Scholar
Leonardi, C and Armentano, LE 2003. Effect of quantity, quality, and length of alfalfa hay on selective consumption by dairy cows. Journal of Dairy Science 86, 557564.Google Scholar
Llonch P, Somarriba M, Duthie CA, Troy S, Roehe R, Rooke J, Haskell MJ and Turner SP 2018. Temperament and dominance relate to feeding behaviour and activity in beef cattle: implications for performance and methane emissions. animal 1-10, doi:10.1017/S1751731118000617 Google Scholar
Llonch, P, Somarriba, M, Duthie, C-A, Haskell, MJ, Rooke, JA, Troy, S, Roehe, R and Turner, SP 2016. Association of temperament and acute stress responsiveness with productivity, feed efficiency, and methane emissions in beef cattle: an observational study. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 3, 19.Google Scholar
Loerch, SC and Fluharty, FL 1999. Physiological changes and digestive capabilities of newly received feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 77.Google Scholar
Løvendahl, P and Munksgaard, L 2016. An investigation into genetic and phenotypic variation in time budgets and yield of dairy cows. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 408417.Google Scholar
Lynch, JJ, Hinch, GN and Adams, DB 1992. The behaviour of sheep: biological principles and implications for production. CAB International, Wallingford, UK.Google Scholar
Meagher, RK, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2017. Some like it varied: individual differences in preference for feed variety in dairy heifers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 195, 814.Google Scholar
Melin, M, Wiktorsson, H and Norell, L 2005. Analysis of feeding and drinking patterns of dairy cows in two cow traffic situations in automatic milking systems. Journal of Dairy Science 88, 7185.Google Scholar
Michelena, P, Sibbald, AM, Erhard, HW and McLeod, JE 2009. Effects of group size and personality on social foraging: the distribution of sheep across patches. Behavioral Ecology 20, 145152.Google Scholar
Miller-Cushon, EK and DeVries, TJ 2011. Effect of early feed type exposure on diet-selection behavior of dairy calves. Journal of Dairy Science 94, 342350.Google Scholar
Miller-Cushon, EK and DeVries, TJ 2015. Invited review: development and expression of dairy calf feeding behaviour. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 95, 341350.Google Scholar
Miller-Cushon EK and DeVries TJ 2016. Effect of social housing on the development of feeding behavior and social feeding preferences of dairy calves. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 1406–1417. Google Scholar
Miller, K and Wood-Gush, DGM 1991. Some effects of housing on the social behaviour of dairy cows. Animal Production 53, 271278.Google Scholar
Miranda-de la Lama, GC and Mattiello, S 2010. The importance of social behaviour for goat welfare in livestock farming. Small Ruminant Research 90, 110.Google Scholar
Miranda-de la Lama, GC, Sepúlveda, WS, Montaldo, HH, María, GA and Galindo, F 2011. Social strategies associated with identity profiles in dairy goats. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 134, 4855.Google Scholar
Nawroth, C, von Borell, E and Langbein, J 2015. ‘Goats that stare at men’: dwarf goats alter their behaviour in response to human head orientation, but do not spontaneously use head direction as a cue in a food-related context. Animal Cognition 18, 6573.Google Scholar
Neave, HW, Costa, JHC, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2018. Personality is associated with feeding behavior and performance in dairy calves. Journal of Dairy Science 101, 7437–7449.Google Scholar
Nielsen G 1999. On the interpretation of feeding behaviour measures and the use of feeding rate as an indicator of social constraint. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 63, 79–91. Google Scholar
Neisen, G, Wechsler, B and Gygax, L 2009. Effects of the introduction of single heifers or pairs of heifers into dairy-cow herds on the temporal and spatial associations of heifers and cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 119, 127136.Google Scholar
Nicol, CJ 1995. The social transmission of information and behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 44, 7998.Google Scholar
Nicol, AM and Sharafeldin, MA 1975. Observations on the behaviour of single-suckled calves from birth to 120 days. Proceeding of the New Zealand Society of Animal Production 35, 221230.Google Scholar
Nkrumah, JD, Crews, DH, Basarab, JA, Price, MA, Okine, EK, Wang, Z, Li, C and Moore, SS 2007. Genetic and phenotypic relationships of feeding behavior and temperament with performance, feed efficiency, ultrasound, and carcass merit of beef cattle. Journal of Animal Science 85, 2382.Google Scholar
Patison, KP, Swain, DL, Bishop-Hurley, GJ, Pattison, P and Robins, G 2010. Social companionship versus food: the effect of the presence of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics on the distance steers travel. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 122, 1320.Google Scholar
Petherick, JC, Holroyd, RG, Doogan, VJ and Venus, BK 2002. Productivity, carcass and meat quality of lot-fed Bos indicus cross steers grouped according to temperament. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 42, 389.Google Scholar
Prins, H 1996. Ecology and behaviour of the African Buffalo: Social inequality and decision making. Chapman & Hall, London, UK.Google Scholar
Proudfoot, K and Habing, G 2015. Social stress as a cause of diseases in farm animals: current knowledge and future directions. The Veterinary Journal 206, 1521.Google Scholar
Provenza, FD 1995. Postingestive feedback as an elementary determinant of food preference and intake in ruminants. Journal of Range Management 48, 217.Google Scholar
Provenza, FD and Balph, DF 1987. Diet learning by domestic ruminants: theory, evidence and practical implications. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 18, 211232.Google Scholar
Provenza, FD and Burritt, EA 1991. Socially induced diet preference ameliorates conditioned food aversion in lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 31, 229236.Google Scholar
Provenza, FD, Villalba, JJ, Dziba, LE, Atwood, SB and Banner, RE 2003. Linking herbivore experience, varied diets, and plant biochemical diversity. Small Ruminant Research 49, 257274.Google Scholar
Ralphs, MH, Graham, D and James, LF 1994. Social facilitation influences cattle to graze locoweed. Journal of Range Management 47, 123126.Google Scholar
Ramseyer, A, Boissy, A, Thierry, B and Dumont, B 2009. Individual and social determinants of spontaneous group movements in cattle and sheep. Animal 3, 13191326.Google Scholar
Rault, J-L, Lay, DC and Marchant-Forde, JN 2011. Castration induced pain in pigs and other livestock. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 135, 214225.Google Scholar
Raussi, S, Niskanen, S, Siivonen, J, , L, Hepola, H, Jauhiainen, L and Veissier, I 2010. The formation of preferential relationships at early age in cattle. Behavioural Processes 84, 726731.Google Scholar
Reinhardt, V and Reinhardt, A 1981. Natural sucking performance and age of weaning in zebu cattle (Bos indicus). Journal of Agricultural Science 96, 309312.Google Scholar
Rice, M, Jongman, EC, Borg, S, Butler, KL and Hemsworth, PH 2016a. Characterisation of Shy-feeding and Feeding lambs in the first week in a feedlot. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 179, 3945.Google Scholar
Rice, M, Jongman, EC, Butler, KL and Hemsworth, PH 2016b. Relationships between temperament, feeding behaviour, social interactions, and stress in lambs adapting to a feedlot environment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 183, 4250.Google Scholar
Rioja-Lang, FC, Roberts, DJ, Healy, SD, Lawrence, AB and Haskell, MJ 2009. Dairy cows trade-off feed quality with proximity to a dominant individual in Y-maze choice tests. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 117, 159164.Google Scholar
Rohrer, GA, Brown-Brandl, T, Rempel, LA, Schneider, JF and Holl, J 2013. Genetic analysis of behavior traits in swine production. Livestock Science 157, 2837.Google Scholar
Ros-Freixedes, R, Sadler, LJ, Onteru, SK, Smith, RM, Young, JM, Johnson, AK, Lonergan, SM, Huff-Lonergan, E, Dekkers, JCM and Rothschild, MF 2014. Relationship between gilt behavior and meat quality using principal component analysis. Meat Science 96, 264269.Google Scholar
Roth, BA, Keil, NM, Gygax, L and Hillmann, E 2009. Influence of weaning method on health status and rumen development in dairy calves. Journal of Dairy Science 92, 645656.Google Scholar
Rushen, J, Taylor, AA and de Passillé, AM 1999. ‘Domestic animals’ fear of humans and its effect on their welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 65, 285303.Google Scholar
Rutter, SM 2006. Diet preference for grass and legumes in free-ranging domestic sheep and cattle: current theory and future application. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 97, 1735.Google Scholar
Sárová, R, Spinka, M, Arias Panamá, JL and Sime Cek, P 2010. Graded leadership by dominant animals in a herd of female beef cattle on pasture. Animal Behaviour 79, 10371045.Google Scholar
Schwartzkopf-Genswein, KS, Atwood, S and McAllister, TA 2002. Relationships between bunk attendance, intake and performance of steers and heifers on varying feeding regimes. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76, 179188.Google Scholar
Sibbald, AM, Erhard, HW, Hooper, RJ, Dumont, B and Boissy, A 2005. A test for measuring individual variation in how far grazing animals will move away from a social group to feed. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 98, 8999.Google Scholar
Sibbald, AM, Erhard, HW, McLeod, JE and Hooper, RJ 2009. Individual personality and the spatial distribution of groups of grazing animals: an example with sheep. Behavioural Processes 82, 319326.Google Scholar
Sibbald, AM and Hooper, RJ 2003. Trade-offs between social behaviour and foraging by sheep in heterogeneous pastures. Behavioural Processes 61, 112.Google Scholar
Sibbald, AM and Hooper, RJ 2004. Sociability and the willingness of individual sheep to move away from their companions in order to graze. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 86, 5162.Google Scholar
Sih, A and Bell, AM 2008. Insights for behavioral ecology from behavioral syndromes. Advances in the Study of Behavior 38, 227281.Google Scholar
Sloan Wilson, D, Clark, AB, Coleman, K and Dearstyne, T 1994. Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 9, 442446.Google Scholar
Squires, VR and Daws, GT 1975. Leadership and dominance relationships in Merino and Border Leicester sheep. Applied Animal. Ethology 1, 263274.Google Scholar
Stears, K, Kerley, GIH and Shrader, AM 2014. Group-living herbivores weigh up food availability and dominance status when making patch-joining decisions. PLoS ONE 9, e109011.Google Scholar
Thouless, CR 1990. Feeding competition between grazing red deer hinds. Animal Behaviour 40, 105111.Google Scholar
Tolkamp, BJ, Kyriazakis, I, Oldham, JD, Lewis, M, Dewhurst, RJ and Newbold, JR 1998. Diet choice by dairy cows. 2. Selection for metabolizable protein or for ruminally degradable protein? Journal of Dairy Science 81, 26702680.Google Scholar
USDA 2014. Dairy 2014. Dairy cattle management practices in the United States. Fort Collins, CO, USA.Google Scholar
Val-Laillet, D, Passillé, AM de, Rushen, J and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2008a. The concept of social dominance and the social distribution of feeding-related displacements between cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 111, 158172.Google Scholar
Val-Laillet, D, Veira, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2008b. Short communication: dominance in free-stall-housed dairy cattle is dependent upon resource. Journal of Dairy Science 91, 39223926.Google Scholar
Van Reenen, CG, O’Connell, NE, Van Der Werf, JTN, Korte, SM, Hopster, H, Jones, RB and Blokhuis, HJ 2005. Responses of calves to acute stress: individual consistency and relations between behavioral and physiological measures. Physiology and Behavior 85, 557570.Google Scholar
Van Reenen, CG, Van der Werf, JTN, Bruckmaier, RM, Hopster, H, Engel, B, Noordhuizen, JPTM and Blokhuis, HJ 2002. Individual differences in behavioral and physiological responsiveness of primiparous dairy cows to machine milking. Journal of Dairy Science 85, 25512561.Google Scholar
Vasseur, E, Borderas, F, Cue, RI, Lefebvre, D, Pellerin, D, Rushen, J, Wade, KM and de Passillé, a M 2010. A survey of dairy calf management practices in Canada that affect animal welfare. Journal of Dairy Science 93, 13071315.Google Scholar
Veissier, I and Stefanova, I 1993. Learning to suckle from an artificial teat within groups of lambs: Influence of a knowledgeable partner. Behavioural Processes 30, 7582.Google Scholar
Villalba, JJ, Catanese, F, Provenza, FD and Distel, RA 2012. Relationships between early experience to dietary diversity, acceptance of novel flavors, and open field behavior in sheep. Physiology and Behavior 105, 181187.Google Scholar
Villalba, JJ, Manteca, X and Provenza, FD 2009. Relationship between reluctance to eat novel foods and open-field behavior in sheep. Physiology and Behavior 96, 276281.Google Scholar
von Keyserlingk, MA. and Weary, DM 2010. Review: Feeding behaviour of dairy cattle: Measures and applications. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 90, 303309.Google Scholar
Warmington, BG and Kirton, AH 1990. Genetic and non-genetic influences on growth and carcass traits of goats. Small Ruminant Research 3, 147165.Google Scholar
Webb, LE, Engel, B, Berends, H, van Reenen, CG, Gerrits, WJJ, de Boer, IJM and Bokkers, EAM 2014. What do calves choose to eat and how do preferences affect behaviour? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 161, 719.Google Scholar
Whalin, L, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2018. Short communication: pair housing dairy calves in modified calf hutches. Journal of Dairy Science 101, 5428–5433.Google Scholar
Zentall, T and Galef, B 1988. Social learning: psychological and biological perspectives. Psychology Press, New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Zobel, G, Schwartzkopf-Genswein, KS, Genswein, BMA and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2011. Impact of agonistic interactions on feeding behaviours when beef heifers are fed in a competitive feeding environment. Livestock Science 137, 19.Google Scholar