Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-24T04:53:37.922Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Too hungry to learn? Hungry broiler breeders fail to learn a Y-maze food quantity discrimination task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

LA Buckley*
Affiliation:
Animals Department, Harper Adams University College, Newport, Shropshire TF10 8NB, UK Scottish Agricultural College, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
LM McMillan
Affiliation:
Scottish Agricultural College, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
V Sandilands
Affiliation:
Scottish Agricultural College, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
BJ Tolkamp
Affiliation:
Scottish Agricultural College, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
PM Hocking
Affiliation:
The Roslin Institute and Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies, Roslin BioCentre, Midlothian EH25 9PS, UK
RB D’Eath
Affiliation:
Scottish Agricultural College, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
*
* Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: lbuckley@harper-adams.ac.uk

Abstract

Choice tests may aid determining whether qualitative dietary restriction improves the welfare of feed-restricted broiler breeder chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus). However, hunger-stress may reduce competency to choose by impairing learning. The effect of chronic feed restriction on the ability of broiler breeders to learn a hunger-relevant discrimination task was investigated using a Y-maze paradigm. The task was to associate black and white arms with large and small quantities of feed. Birds were reared to three growth curves by means of severe (n = 12), moderate (n = 12) or very mild feed restriction (n = 12). Learning the task and selecting the larger food option allowed birds to increase their feed intake. Time taken to traverse the Y-maze was also measured. Birds from all treatment groups traversed the Y-maze more quickly over time, indicating that they had learnt that running down the Y-maze arms was associated with a rewarding outcome (food). However, feed restriction significantly reduced their ability to associate the black and white cues with differences in food quantity. Consequently, average pay-offs in terms of daily feed increments disproportionately accrued to the less feed-restricted treatment groups. It is concluded that feed restriction affected the performance of broiler breeders in this task, perhaps by narrowing their attention such that they ignore potentially hunger-relevant contextual cues. However, low overall group success rates demonstrate that this task was difficult to learn even for less severely feed-restricted birds. Therefore, Y-maze choice tests may not be the most appropriate method for determining hungry broiler breeder dietary preferences.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2011 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aviagen 2006 Ross 308: Parent Stock Management Manual pp 186. Aviagen: Midlothian, UKGoogle Scholar
Aviagen 2007 Ross 308: Parent Stock Performance Objectives pp 119. Aviagen: Midlothian, UKGoogle Scholar
Baghbanzadeh, A and Decuypere, E 2008 Ascites syndrome in broilers: physiological and nutritional perspectives. Avian Pathology 37: 117126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckley, LA, Sandilands, V, Tolkamp, BJ and D’Eath, RB 2010 Quantifying Hungry Broiler Breeder Dietary Preferences using a Closed Economy T-Maze Task. Proceedings of the 44th Congress for the International Society of Applied Ethology p 232. 4-7 August 2010, International Society for Applied Ethology, Uppsala, SwedenGoogle Scholar
Capaldi, EJ and Molina, P 1979 Element discriminability as a determinant of serial-pattern learning. Animal Learning and Behavior 7: 318322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, KN 1964 T-maze choice learning as a joint function of the reward magnitudes for the alternatives. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology 58: 333338CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conrad, CD 2010 A critical review of chronic stress effects on spatial learning and memory. Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 34: 742755CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Amato, MR 1960 Distribution variables in simple discrimination learning in rats. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 14: 216219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Eath, RB, Tolkamp, BJ, Kyriazakis, I and Lawrence, AB 2009 ‘Freedom from hunger’ and preventing obesity: the animal welfare implications of reducing food quantity or quality. Animal Behaviour 77: 275288Google Scholar
Dawkins, MS 1990 From an animals’ point of view: motivation, fitness, and animal welfare. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jong, IC, Enting, H, van Voorst, AS and Blokhuis, HJ 2005 Do low density diets improve broiler breeder welfare during rearing and laying? Poultry Science 84: 194203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong, IC, van Voorst, AS and Blokhuis, HJ 2003 Parameters for quantification of hunger in broiler breeders. Physiology and Behaviour 78: 773783CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong, IC, van Voorst, AS, Ehlhardt, DA and Blokhuis, HJ 2002 Effects of restricted feeding on physiological stress parameters in growing broiler breeders. British Poultry Science 43: 157168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deng, L, Wu, ZN and Han, PZ 2009 Effects of different levels of food restriction on passive-avoidance memory and the expression of synapsin 1 in young mice. International Journal of Neuroscience 119: 291304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diano, S, Farr, SA, Benoit, SC, McNay, EC, da, SI, Horvath, B, Gaskin, FS, Nonaka, N, Jaeger, LB, Banks, WA, Morley, JE, Pinto, S, Sherwin, RS, Xu, L, Yamada, KA, Sleeman, MW, Tschop, MH and Horvath, TL 2006 Ghrelin controls hippocampal spine synapse density and memory performance. Natural Neuroscience 9: 381388CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Easterbrook, JA 1959 The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review 66: 183201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisman, E, Asimow, A and Maltzman, I 1956 Habit strength as a function of drive in a brightness discrimination problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology 52: 5864CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, FR, Spini, VBMG, Lopes, EJ, Renata, FF, Moreira, EA, Amaral, MAF, Cunha, ALLS, Borges, APS, Marson, LB and Ribeiro, GCC 2006 Effect of feed restriction on learning, memory and stress in rodents. Bioscience Journal 22: 9197Google Scholar
Haskell, MJ, Forkman, B and Waddington, D 1998 An investigation into the occurrence of spontaneous alternation behaviour in the domestic hen. Behavioural Processes 43: 4351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, WF and Spear, NE 1963 Choice between magnitudes of reward in a T-maze. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology 56: 723726CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hocking, PM 2006 High-fibre pelleted rations decrease water intake but do not improve physiological indices of welfare in food-restricted female broiler breeders. British Poultry science 47: 1923CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hocking, PM, Maxwell, MH and Mitchell, MA 1993 Welfare assessment for broiler breeder and layer females subjected to food restriction and limited access to water during rearing. British Poultry Science 34: 443458CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hocking, PM, Maxwell, MH and Mitchell, MA 1996 Relationships between the degree of feed restriction and welfare indices in female broiler breeders. British Poultry Science 37: 263278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hocking, PM, Zaczek, V, Jones, EKM and Macleod, MG 2004 Different concentrations and sources of dietary fibre may improve the welfare of female broiler breeders. British Poultry Science 37: 263278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, RN 1989 Lack of spontaneous-alternation in favor of perseveration in domestic fowls and pigeons. Behavioural Processes 20: 8592CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joels, M, Pu, ZW, Wiegert, O, Oitzl, MS and Krugers, HJ 2006 Learning under stress: how does it work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10: 152158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kestin, SC, Gordon, S, Su, G and Sorenson, P 2001 Relationships in broiler chickens between lameness, liveweight, growth rate and age. Veterinary Record 148: 195197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levitsky, DA 1979 Malnutrition and the hunger to learn. In: Levitsky, DA (ed) Malnutrition, Environment and Behaviour pp 161179. Cornell University Press: Ithaca, NY, USAGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M 1999 Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive function. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 65: 221244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michels, B, Diegelmann, S, Tanimoto, H, Schwenkert, I, Buchner, E and Gerber, B 2005 A role for synapsin in associative learning: the Drosophila larva as a study case. Learning and Memory 12: 224231CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, D, Osborne, EW and Oboyle, MW 1985 Habituation under stress: shocked mice show non-associative learning in a T-Maze. Behavioral and Neural Biology 43: 212217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, BL, Litherland, M and Noddegaard, F 2003 Effects of qualitative and quantitative feed restriction on the activity of broiler chickens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 83: 309323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicol, CJ and Pope, SJ 1993 Food deprivation during observation reduces social learning in hens. Animal Behaviour 45: 193196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrie, A & Watson, P 1999 Statistics for Veterinary and Animal Science. Blackwell Science Ltd: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Phillips, CJ and Strojan, ST 2007 The ability of chickens to select nutritive and avoid toxic concentrations of heavy metals in feeds. Journal of Sustainable Agriculture 30: 3145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porton, B, Rodriguiz, RM, Phillips, LE, Gilbert JW, IV, Feng, J, Greengard, P, Kao, HT and Wetsel, WC 2010 Mice lacking synapsin III show abnormalities in explicit memory and conditioned fear. Genes, Brain and Behaviour 9: 257268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richman, CL, Gardner, JT, Montgomery, MD and Benewicz, KL 1970 Effects of body weight loss on position and brightness discrimination tasks. Learning and Motivation 1: 218225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, FE and Wilson, JL 1996 Reproductive failure in overweight male and female broiler breeders. Animal Feed Science and Technology 58: 143150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, PJ, Tonkiss, J and Smart, JL 1986 Incidental-learning is impaired during early-life undernutrition. Developmental Psychobiology 19: 113124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozenboim, I, Kapkowska, E, Robinzon, B and Uni, Z 1999 Effects of fenfluramine on body weight, feed intake and reproductive activities of broiler breeder hens. Poultry Science 78: 17681772CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandi, C, Loscertales, M and Guaza, C 1997 Experience-dependent facilitating effect of corticosterone on spatial memory formation in the water maze. The European Journal of Neuroscience 9: 637642CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandilands, V, Tolkamp, BJ and Kyriazakis, I 2005 Behaviour of food restricted broilers during rearing and lay: effects of an alternative feeding method. Physiology and Behaviour 85: 115123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandilands, V, Tolkamp, BJ, Savory, CJ and Kyriazakis, I 2006 Behaviour and welfare of broiler breeders fed qualitatively restricted diets during rearing: Are there viable alternatives to quantitative restriction? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 96: 5367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarason, IG, Sarason, BR, Miller, M and Mahmoud, P 1956 The role of the intertrial interval in discrimination and reversal learning. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology 49: 7779CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savory, CJ, Hocking, PM, Mann, JS and Maxwell, MH 1996 Is broiler breeder welfare improved by using qualitative orather than quantitative food restriction to limit growth rate? Animal Welfare 5: 105127Google Scholar
Savory, CJ and Lariviere, JM 2000 Effect of qualitative and quantitative food restriction treatments on feeding motivational state and general activity level of growing broiler breeders. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 69: 135147CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savory, CJ and Maros, K 1993 Influence of degree of food restriction, age and time of day on behavior of broiler breeder chickens. Behavioural Processes 29: 179190CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savory, CJ, Maros, K and Rutter, SM 1993 Assessment of hunger in growing broiler breeders in relation to a commercial breeding programme. Animal Welfare 2: 131152Google Scholar
Schwabe, L and Wolf, OT 2009 Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 29: 71917198CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwabe, L and Wolf, OT 2010 Learning under stress impairs memory formation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 93: 183188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwabe, L, Wolf, OT and Oitzl, MS 2010 Memory formation under stress: quantity and quality. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34: 584591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strupp, BJ and Levitsky, DA 1995 Enduring cognitive effects of early malnutrition: a theoretical reappraisal. The Journal of Nutrition 125(8S): 22212232CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talling, JC, Inglis, IR, Van Driel, KS, Young, J and Giles, S 2002 Effect of hunger on starlings’ preferences for food sources associated with variability or uncertainty. Behaviour 139: 12231235Google Scholar
Van Rooijen, J and Metz, J 1987 A preliminary experiment on T-maze choice tests. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 19: 5156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vermaut, S, De Coninck, K, Bruggeman, V, Onagbesan, O, Flo, G, Cokelaere, M and Decuypere, E 1999 Evaluation of jojoba meal as a potential supplement in the diet of broiler breeder females during laying. British Poultry Science 40: 284291CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yerkes, RM and Dodson, JD 1908 The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology 18: 459482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, GS and Kirkland, JB 2007 Rat models of caloric intake and activity: relationships to animal physiology and human health. Applied Physiology, Nutrition, and Metabolism 32: 161176CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuidhof, MJ, Robinson, FE, Feddes, FE, Hardin, RT, Wilson, JL, McKay, RI and Newcombe, M 1995 The effects of nutrient dilution on the well-being and and performance of female broiler breeders. Poultry Science 74: 441456CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed