Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-qs9v7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T22:18:28.187Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Language in Down syndrome: A life-span perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Marcia A. Barnes
Affiliation:
University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston
Get access

Summary

Down syndrome (DS), or trisomy 21, is divided according to three etiological subcategories: (1) standard trisomy, (2) mosaicism, and (3) translocation. In 97% of all cases (the standard trisomy 21 subcategory), the genetic error takes place in the ovula or the spermatozoid before syngamy or during the first cell division. All the living cells of the embryo receive three chromosome 21s. In 1% of all cases (Hamerton et al., 1965; 2% according to Richards, 1969; the mosaicism subcatregory), the genetic error takes place during the second or the third cell division. In those cases, the embryo develops with a mosaic of normal cells containing the regular number of 46 chromosomes and cells with three chromosome 21s. In the remaining 2% (1%, according to Richards) of all cases (the translocation subcategory), the additional chromosomic material is not a triplicate of chromosome 21 but a part or the totality of another chromosome (often chromosome 14 or 22). In about 66% of the translocation cases, the genetic error takes place during the formation of the ovula or the spermatozoid, or during the first division of the embryo cell. In 34% of the cases, one of the parents, although phenotypically normal in all respects, carries the translocation in his/her genotype.

Type
Chapter
Information
Genes, Brain and Development
The Neurocognition of Genetic Disorders
, pp. 122 - 142
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbeduto, L., & Rosenberg, S. (1980). The communicative competence of mildly retarded adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 405–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbeduto, L., Davies, B., & Furman, L. (1988). The development of speech act comprehension in mentally retarded individuals and nonretarded children. Child Development, 59, 1460–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abbeduto, L., Davies, B., Solesby, S., & Furman, L. (1991). Identifying the referents of spoken messages: Use of context and clarification request by children with and without mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 95, 551–62.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (1980). Human Memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barrett, M. & Diniz, F. (1989). Lexical development in mentally handicapped children. In Beveridge, M., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Leudar, I. (Eds.), Language and Communication in Mentally Handicapped People (pp. 3–32). London: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Benda, C. (1949). Mongolism and Cretinism. New York: Grune & Startton.Google Scholar
Berry, P., Groenweg, G., Gibson, D., & Brown, R. (1984). Mental development of adults with Down's syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 89, 252–6.Google Scholar
Bray, M. & Woolnough, L. (1988). The language of children with Down's syndrome aged 12 to 16 years. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 4, 311–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckley, S. & Sacks, B. (1987). The adolescent with Down's syndrome –Life for the teenager teaching and the family. Portsmouth, UK: Portsmouth Down's syndrome Trust.Google Scholar
Burt, D., Loveland, K., Chen, Y.-W., Chuang, A., Lewis, K., & Cherry, L. (1995). Ageing in adults with Down syndrome: Report from a longitudinal study. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 100, 262–70.Google ScholarPubMed
Chapman, R. (1995). Language development in children and adolescents with Down's syndrome. In Fletcher, P. & McWhinney, B. (Eds.), The Handbook of Child Language (pp. 641–63). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. (2003). Language and communication in individuals with Down syndrome. In Abbeduto, L. (Ed.), Language and Communication in Mental Retardation (pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, R., Schwartz, S., & Kay-Raining Bird, E. (1991). Language skills of children and adolescents with Down's syndrome: I. Comprehension. Journal Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1106–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, R., Schwartz, S., & Kay-Raining Bird, E. (1992, August). Language production of children and adolescents with Down's syndrome. Paper presented at the 9th World Congress of the International Association for the Scientific Study of Mental Deficiency. Gold Coast: Australia.Google Scholar
Chapman, R., Seung, H., Schwartz, S., & Kay-Raining Bird, E. (1998). Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: Production deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41, 861–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, C. M., Edwards, J. H., & Smallpeice, V. (1961). Trisomy 21 normal mosaicism in an intelligent child with some mongoloid characters. Lancet, 1028–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coggins, T. (1979). Relational meaning encoded in two-word utterance of stage 1 in Down's syndrome children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 166–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comblain, A. (1994). Working memory in Down's syndrome: Training rehearsal strategy. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 2, 123–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comblain, A. (1995). Batterie pour l'évaluation de la morpho-syntaxe. Unpublished manuscript, University of Liège, Belgium.
Comblain, A. (1996). Mémoire de travail et langage dans le syndrome de Down. Unpublished doctoral dissertation (Logopedics), University of Liège, Belgium.
Cunningham, C. & Sloper, P. (1984). The relationship between maternal ratings of first word vocabulary and Reynell language scores. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 160–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtiss, S. (1989). Abnormal language acquisition and the modularity of language. In Newmeyer, F., (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey (Vol. 2, pp. 96–116). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Das, J. P., Divis, B., Alexander, J., Parrila, R., & Naglieri, J. (1995). Cognitive decline due to aging among persons with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 16, 461–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devenny, D., Hill, A., Patxot, D., Silverman, W., & Wisniewsky, H. (1992). Ageing in higher functioning adults with Down's syndrome: A longitudinal study. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 36, 241–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devenny, D., Silverman, W., Hill, A., Jenkins, E., Sersen, E., & Wisniewski, H. (1996). Normal ageing in adult with Down's syndrome: A longitudinal study. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40, 208–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, B. (1976). A comparison of the phonological systems of mental-age-matched severely subnormal and Down's syndrome children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 11, 27–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, B. & Leahy, Y. (1989). Phonological disorders and mental handicap. In Beveridge, M., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Leudar, Y. (Eds.), Language and Communication in Mentally Handicapped People (pp. 33–56). London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Duchan, J. F. & Erickson, J. G. (1976). Normal and nonretarded children's understanding of semantic relations in different verbals contexts. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 767–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenner, M., Hewitt, K., & Torpy, D. (1987). Down's syndrome: Intellectual and behavioural functioning during adulthood. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 31, 241–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Fishler, K. & Koch, R. (1991). Mental development in Down's syndrome mosaicism. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 96, 345–51.Google ScholarPubMed
Fowler, A. (1988). Determinants of rate of language growth in children with Down's syndrome. In Nadel, L (Ed.), The Psychobiology of Down's Syndrome (pp. 217–45). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fowler, A., Gelman, R., & Gleitman, L. (1994). The course of language learning in children with Down's syndrome. In Tager-Flusberg, H (Ed.), Constraints on Language Acquisition. Studies of Atypical Children (pp. 91–140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. & Baddeley, A. (1993). Working Memory and Language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
George, M., Thewis, B., Linden, M., Salmon, E., & Rondal, J. A. (2001). Elaboration d'une batterie d'évaluation des fonctions cognitives de sujets âgés porteurs d'un syndrome de Down. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 11(4), 549–79.Google Scholar
Gibson, D. (1981). Down's syndrome: The Psychology of Mongolism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. (1985). An Introduction to Functional Grammar. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Hamerton, J., Giannelli, F., & Polani, P. (1965). Cytogenetics of Down's syndrome (mongolism) I. Data on consecutive series of patients referred for genetic counselling and diagnosis. Cytogenetics, 4, 171–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodapp, R. & Dykens, E. (2004). Genetic and behavioural aspects: Application to maladaptive behaviour and cognition. In Rondal, J. A, Hodapp, R, Soresi, S., Dykens, E., & Nota, L., Intellectual Disabilities. Genetics, Behaviour and Inclusion (pp. 13–48). London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Jarrold, C. & Baddeley, A. (1997). Short-term memory for verbal and visuospatial information in Down's syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 101–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A., & Phillips, C. (1999). Down syndrome and the phonological loop: The evidence for an important verbal short-term memory deficit. Down Syndrom Research and Practice, 6, 61–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. & Kaufman, N. (1993). Batterie pour l'Examen Psychologique de l'Enfant. Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.Google Scholar
Layton, T. & Sharifi, H. (1979). Meaning and structure of Down's syndrome and nonretarded children spontaneous speech. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 83, 439–445.Google ScholarPubMed
Leifer, J. & Lewis (1984). Acquisition of conversational response skills by young Down syndrome and nonretarded young children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 88, 610–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E., Nichols, L., & Rosenberger, E. (1964). Primitive stages of development in mongolism. In McRioch, D. & Weinstein, A. (Eds.), Disorders of Communication (pp. 119–137). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Mackenzie, S. & Hulme, C. (1987). Memory span development in Down'syndrome, severely subnormal and normal subjects. Cognitive Neurospsychology, 4, 303–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcell, M. & Armstrong, V. (1982). Auditory and visual sequential memory of Down syndrome and nonretarded children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 87, 86–95.Google ScholarPubMed
Marcell, M. & Weeks, S. (1988). Short-term memory difficulties and Down's syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 32, 153–62.Google ScholarPubMed
Mayberry, R., Fisher, S., & Hatfield, N. (1983). Sentence repetition in American Sign Language. In Kyle, J. & Woll, B. (Eds.), Language in Sign: International Perspective on Sign Language (pp. 206–214). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Menn, L. (1983). Development of articulatory, phonetic, and phonological capabilities. In Butterworth, B. (Ed.), Language Production (Vol. 2, pp. 3–50). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. (1987). Child-basic object categories and early lexical development. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization (pp. 201–33). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. & Becerra, A. (2003). Lexical development and intervention. In Rondal, J. A. & Buckley, S. (Eds.), Speech and Language Intervention in Down Syndrome (pp. 63–85). London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category principle. Child Development, 65, 1646–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. & Bertrand, J. (1995). Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category principle by young children who have Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 100, 231–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Montague, J., & Hollien, H. (1973). Perceived voice quality disorders in Down's syndrome children. Journal of Human Communication Disorders, 6, 76–87.CrossRef
Moss, S., Tomoeda, C., & Bayles, K. (2000). Comparison of the cognitive linguistic profiles of Down syndrome adults with and without dementia to individuals with Alzeimer's desease. Journal of Medical Speech Language Pathology, 8, 69–81.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Seibert, J., & Hogan, A. (1984). Relationship between sensorimotor and early communication abilities in developmentally delayed children. Merill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 33–48.Google Scholar
Newport, E. (1992). Contrasting conception of the critical period for language. In Carey, S. & Gelman, R. (Eds.), The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition (pp. 11–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Owings, N., McManus, M., & Scherer, N. (1981). A deinstitutionalized retarded adult's use of communication functions in a natural setting, British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 16, 119–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ploog, D. (1984). Comment on J. Leiber's paper. In Harre, R. & Reynolds, V. (Eds.), The Meaning of Primate Signals (pp. 88). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Prasher, V. (1996). Age-associated functional decline in adults with Down's syndrome. European Journal of Psychiatry, 10, 129–35.Google Scholar
Pueschel, S. (1995). Caracteristicas fisicas de las personas con syndrome de Down. In Perera, J. (Ed.), Especificidad en el sindrome de Down (pp. 53–63). Barcelona: Masson.Google Scholar
Reilly, J., Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1991). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development Psychopathology, 2, 367–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ribes, R. & Sanny, J. (2000). Declive cognitivo en memoria y lenguaje: Indicadores del proceso de envejecimiento psicologico en la persona con sindrome de Down. Revista Sindrome de Down, 17, 54–9.Google Scholar
Richards, B. W. (1969). Mosaic mongolism. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 13, 66–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rondal, J. A. (1975). Développement du langage et retard mental: Une revue critique de la littérature en langue anglaise. L'Année Psychologique, 75, 513–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1978). Maternal speech to normal and Down's syndrome children matched for mean length of utterance. In Meyers, E. (Ed.), Quality of Life in Severely and Profoundly Mentally Retarded People: Research Foundations for Improvement (pp. 193–265). Monograph Series n°3. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Deficiency.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1984). Linguistic development in metal retardation. In Dobbing, J., Clarke, A. D. B., Corbett, J., Hog, J., & Robinson, R. (Eds.), Scientific Studies in Mental Retardation (pp. 323–345). London: The Royal Society of Medicine and Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1985). Adult-Child Interactions and the Process of Language Acquisition. New York: Praeger.
Rondal, J. A. (1995). Exceptional Language Development in Down Syndrome. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (2003). Atypical language development in individual with mental retardation: Theoretical implications. In Abbeduto, L. (Ed.), Language and Communication in Mental Retardation. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation (Vol. 27, pp. 281–308). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A. & Comblain, A. (1996). Language in adults with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 4, 3–14.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. & Comblain, A. (2002). Language in ageing persons with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 8(1), 1–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rondal, J. A. & Edwards, S. (1997). Language in mental retardation. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A., Elbouz, M., Ylieff, M., & Docquier, L. (2003). Françoise, a fifteen-year follow up. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 8(3), 89–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rondal, J. A. & Lambert, J. L. (1983). The speech of mentally retarded adults in a dyadic communication situation: Some formal and informative aspects. Psychologica Belgica, 23, 49–56.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A., Lambert, J. L., & Sohier, C. (1980). L'imitation verbale et non verbale chez l'enfant retardé mental mongolien et non mongolien. Enfance, 3, 107–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A., Thibaut, J. P., & Cession, A. (1990). Transitivity effects on children's sentence comprehension. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 10, 385–400.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1978). Human categorization. In Warren, N. (Ed.), Advances in Cross-Cultural Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 122–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, S. & Abbeduto, L. (1993). Language and Communication in Mental Retardation. Development, Processes, and Intervention. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rosin, M., Swift, E., & Bless, D. (1987, May). Communication Profiles of People with Down's Syndrome. Communication presented at the Annual Convention of the American Speech and Hearing Association, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Rosin, M., Swift, E., & Bless, D., & Vetter, D. (1988). Communication profiles of adolescents with Down's syndrome. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 12, 49–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, J. (1975). Mental subnormality and language development. In Lenneberg, E. (Ed.), Foundations of Language Development: A Multidisciplinary Approach (Vol. 2, pp. 269–77). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Scherer, N. & Owings, N. (1984). Learning to be contingent: Retarded children's responses to their mothers' requests. Language and Speech, 27, 255–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, B. & Oller, K. (1981). A comparative study of pre-meaningful vocalizations produced by normally developing and Down's syndrome infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 46–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, B. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (1983). A longitudinal study of the development of stop consonant production in normal and Down's syndrome children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 114–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, R., Rabinowitch, J., & Wybar, K. (1961). A study of the abnormalities of the skull, teeth and lenses in mongolism. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 82, 567–72.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2001). Down syndrome phonology: Developmental patterns and intervention strategies. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 7, 93–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1986). Constraints on the representation of word meaning: Evidence from autistic and mentally retarded children. In Kuczaj, S. & Barrett, M. (Eds.), The Development of Word Meaning (pp. 69–81). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thordadottir, E., Chapman, R., & Wagner, L. (2002). Complex sentence production by adolescents with Down syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(2), 163–83.Google Scholar
Borsel, J. (1988). An analysis of the speech of five Down's syndrome adolescents. Journal of Communication Disorders, 21, 409–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsel, J. (1993). De articulatie bij adolescenten en volwassenen met het syndroom van Down. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Vrije Universiteit Brussels, Brussels.Google Scholar
Buggenhout, G., Lukusa, T., Trommelen, J., Bal, C., Hamel, B., & Fryns, J.-P. (2001). Une étude pluri disciplinaire du syndrome de Down dans une population résidentielle d'arriérés mentaux d'âge avancé: Implications pour le suivi médical. Journal de la Trisomie 21(2), 7–13.Google Scholar
Wilson, B., Ivani-Chalian, R., & Aldrich, F. (1991). The Child Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test. San Antonio, TX: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
Zetlin, A. & Sabsay, S. (1980, Match).Characteristics of verbal interaction among moderately retarded peers. Paper presented at the Gatlinburg Conference on Research in Mental Retardation, Gatlinburg, TN.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×