Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T14:40:52.959Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - A cognitive approach to elicit verbal and nonverbal cues to deceit

from Part IV - Reducing dishonesty

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2016

Jan-Willem van Prooijen
Affiliation:
VU University Amsterdam
Paul A. M. van Lange
Affiliation:
VU University Amsterdam
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Cheating, Corruption, and Concealment
The Roots of Dishonesty
, pp. 284 - 302
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ansarra, R., Colwell, K., Hiscock-Anisman, C., Hines, A., Fleck, R., Cole, L., & Belarde, D. (2011). Augmenting ACID with affective details to assess credibility. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 3, 141158. Doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5547-9_11.Google Scholar
Bagley, J., & Manelis, L. (1979). Effect of awareness on an indicator of cognitive load. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 591594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. W. (1981). A further investigation of the cognitive interference hypothesis of gaze patterns during conversation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 243248. Doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1981.tb00493.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, B. E., & Loftus, E. F. (1989). Trivial persuasion in the courtroom: The power of (a few) minor details. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 669679. Doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.56.5.669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bembibre, J., & Higueras, L. (2011). Differential effectiveness of the cognitive interview in a simulation of a testimony. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 17, 473489. Doi 10.1080/10683160903321540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bembibre, J., & Higueras, L. (2012). Comparative analysis of true and false statements with the source monitoring model and the cognitive interview: Special features of the false accusation of innocent people. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 18, 913928. Doi 10.1080/1068316X.2011.589387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthold, A., & Jameson, A. (1999). Interpreting symptoms of cognitive load in speech input. In Kay, J. (Ed.), User modeling: Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference, UM99, Vienna. New York: Springer Wien New York.Google Scholar
Bull, R. (2010). The investigative interviewing of children and other vulnerable witnesses: Psychological research and working/professional practice. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 15, 524. Doi: 10.1348/014466509X440160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (second edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Colwell, K., Hiscock, C. K., & Memon, A. (2002). Interview techniques and the assessment of statement credibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 287300. Doi: 10.1002/acp.788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colwell, K., Hiscock-Anisman, C. K., Memon, A., Taylor, L., & Prewett, J. (2007). Assessment criteria indicative of deception (ACID): An integrated system of investigative interviewing and detecting deception. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 4, 167180. Doi 10.1002/jip.73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. L., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74118. Doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bruce, V., Bonner, L., Longbotham, S., & Doyle, C. (2002). Development of gaze aversion as disengagement of visual information. Developmental Psychology, 38, 438445. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.38.3.438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Phelps, F. G. (2005). Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty? Memory and Cognition, 33, 727733. doi:10.3758/BF03195338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1997). Deception, lying, and demeanor. In Halpern, D. F. & Voiskounsky, A. E. (Eds.), States of mind: American and post-Soviet perspectives on contemporary issues in psychology (pp. 93105). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1972). Hand movements. Journal of Communication, 22, 353374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, R. P. (2010). Interviewing cooperative witnesses. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 15, 2538 Doi: 10.1348/135532509X441891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, R. P., & Geiselman, R. E. (1992). Memory-enhancing techniques in investigative interviewing: The cognitive interview. Springfield, IL: C.C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Fisher, R., Milne, R., & Bull, R. (2011). Interviewing cooperative witnesses. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 1619. Doi: 10.1177/0963721410396826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geiselman, R. E., & Callot, R. (1990). Reverse and forward order recall of script based text. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 141144. doi 10.1002/acp.2350040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, J. A. E., & Fisher, R. P. (2006). The effects of varied retrieval cues on reminiscence in eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 723739. Doi: 10.1002/acp.1232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L., & Robertson, D. A. (1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory & Cognition, 26, 651658. Doi: 10.3758/BF03211385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Granhag, P. A., Andersson, L. O., Strömwall, L. A., & Hartwig, M. (2004). Imprisoned knowledge: Criminals’ beliefs about deception. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 9, 103119. doi/10.1348/135532504322776889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granhag, P. A., & Hartwig, M. (2008). A new theoretical perspective on deception detection: On the psychology of instrumental mind-reading. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14, 189200. Doi: 10.1080/10683160701645181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granhag, P. A., & Knieps, M. (2011). Episodic future thought: Illuminating the trademarks of forming true and false intentions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 274280. Doi 10.1002/acp1674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granhag, P. A., Strömwall, L. A., & Jonsson, A. C. (2003). Partners in crime: How liars in collusion betray themselves. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 848868. Doi: 10.1111/j.1559–1816.2003.tb01928.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granhag, P.A., Strömwall, L. A., Willén, R., & Hartwig, M. (2013). Eliciting cues to deception by tactical disclosure of evidence: The first test of the Evidence Framing Matrix. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 18, 341355. doi. 10.1111/j.2044–8333.2012.02047.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., & Strömwall, L. (2007). Guilty and innocent suspects’ strategies during interrogations. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 13, 213227. Doi: 10/10683160600750264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., Strömwall, L., & Kronkvist, O. (2006). Strategic use of evidence during police interrogations: When training to detect deception works. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 603619. Doi: org/10.1007/s10979-006-9053-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez-Fernaud, E., & Alonso-Quecuty, M. (1997). The cognitive interview and lie detection: A new magnifying glass for Sherlock Holmes? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 5568. Doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099–0720(199702)11:1<55::AID-ACP423>3.0.CO;2-G.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K. (2006). Memory and reality. American Psychologist, 61, 760771. Doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 371376. Doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.117.4.371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahana, M. J. (1996). Associate retrieval processes in free recall. Memory & Cognition, 24, 103109. Doi: 10.3758/BF03197276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knieps, M., Granhag, P. A., & Vrij, A. (2013a). Back to the future: Asking about mental images to discriminate between true and false intentions. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 147, 619640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knieps, M., Granhag, P. A., & Vrij, A. (2013b). Repeated visits to the future: Asking about mental images to discriminate between true and false intentions. International Journal of Advances in Psychology, 2, 93102.Google Scholar
Köhnken, G. (1996). Social psychology and the law. In Semin, G. R. & Fiedler, K. (Eds.), Applied social psychology (pp. 257282). London, Great Britain: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köhnken, G. (2004). Statement Validity Analysis and the “detection of the truth.” In Granhag, P. A. & Strömwall, L. A. (Eds.), Deception detection in forensic contexts (pp. 4163). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 5459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancaster, G. L. J., Vrij, A., Hope, L., & Waller, B. (2012). Sorting the liars from the truth tellers: The benefits of asking unanticipated questions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 107114. Doi: 10.1002/acp.2879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leal, S., Vrij, A., Warmelink, L., Vernham, Z., & Fisher, R. (2015). You cannot hide your telephone lies: Providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 20, 129146. Doi: 10.1111/lcrp.12017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leins, D., Fisher, R., & Vrij, A. (2012). Drawing on liars’ lack of cognitive flexibility: Detecting deception through varying report modes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 601607. Doi 10.1002/acp.2837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leins, D., Fisher, R. P., Vrij, A., Leal, S., & Mann, S. (2011). Using sketch-drawing to induce inconsistency in liars. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 16, 253265. Doi 10.1348/135532510X501775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, S., Vrij, A., Leal, S., Granhag, P. A., Warmelink, L., & Forrester, D. (2012). Windows to the soul? Deliberate eye contact as a cue to deceit. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 36, 205215. Doi: 10.1007/s10919-012-0132-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, S., Vrij, A., Shaw, D., Leal, S., Ewans, S., Hillman, J., Granhag, P. A., & Fisher, R. P. (2013). Two heads are better than one? How to effectively use two interviewers to elicit cues to deception. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 18, 324340. Doi 10.1111/j.2044-8333.2012.02055.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Memon, A., Meissner, C. A., & Fraser, J. (2010). The cognitive interview: A meta-analytic review and study space analysis of the past 25 years. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 16, 340372. Doi:10.1037/a0020518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nahari, G., Vrij, A., & Fisher, R. P. (2012). Does the truth come out in the writing? SCAN as a lie detection tool. Law & Human Behavior, 36, 6876. Doi: 10.1007/s10979-011-9264-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redick, T. S., Broadway, J. M., Meier, M. E., Kuriakose, P. S., Unsworth, N., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Measuring working memory capacity with automated complex span tasks. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28, 164171. Doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roos af Hjelmsäter, E., Öhman, L., Granhag, P. A., & Vrij, A. (2014). Mapping deception in adolescents: Eliciting cues to deceit through an unanticipated spatial drawing task. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 19, 179188. Doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8333.2012.02068.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1994). Supervisory control of action and thought selection. In Weiskrantz, L., Baddeley, A., & Alan, D. (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness and control: A tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 171187). New York: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. J, Vrij, A., Mann, S., Leal, S., & Hillman, J. (2013). Expect the unexpected? Variations in question type elicit cues to deception in joint interviewer contexts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 336343. Doi: 10.1002/acp.2911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sooniste, T., Granhag, P. A., Knieps, M., & Vrij, A. (2013). True and false intentions: Asking about the past to detect lies about the future. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 19, 673685. Doi 10.1080/1068316x.2013.793333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, S. A., Hunter, M. D., Farrow, T. F. D., Green, R. D., Leung, D. H., & Hughes, C. J. (2004). A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 359, 17551762. Doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1555.Google ScholarPubMed
Steller, M., & Köhnken, G. (1989). Criteria-Based Content Analysis. In Raskin, D. C. (Ed.), Psychological methods in criminal investigation and evidence (pp. 217245). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Suckle-Nelson, J. A., Colwell, K., Hiscock-Anisman, C., Florence, S., Youschak, K. E., & Duarte, A. (2010). Assessment criteria indicative of deception (ACID): Replication and gender differences. The Open Criminology Journal, 3, 2330.Google Scholar
Tedeschini, J. (2012). Overcoming roadblocks to reform. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 134135. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vredeveldt, A., Hitch, G. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2011). Eye closure helps memory by reducing cognitive load and enhancing visualisation. Memory and Cognition, 39, 12531263. Doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0098-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrij, A. (2005). Criteria-Based Content Analysis: A qualitative review of the first 37 studies. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 341. Doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.11.1.3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities, second edition. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Vrij, A. (2014). Interviewing to detect deception. European Psychologist, 19, 184195. Doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A. (2015). A cognitive approach to lie detection. In Granhag, P. A., Vrij, A., & Verschuere, B. (Eds.), Deception detection: Current challenges and new approaches (pp. 205229). Chichester, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
Vrij, A., Fisher, R., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2006). Detecting deception by manipulating cognitive load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 141142. Doi: org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrij, A., & Granhag, P. A. (2012). Eliciting cues to deception and truth: What matters are the questions asked. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 110117. doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.02.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2011). Outsmarting the liars: Towards a cognitive lie detection approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 2832. Doi: 10.1177/0963721410391245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., & Porter, S. B. (2010). Pitfalls and opportunities in nonverbal and verbal lie detection. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11, 89121. Doi: 10.1177/1529100610390861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrij, A., Leal, S., Granhag, P. A., Mann, S., Fisher, R. P., Hillman, J., & Sperry, K. (2009). Outsmarting the liars: The benefit of asking unanticipated questions. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 159166. Doi 10.1007/s10979-008-9143-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrij, A., Leal, S., Mann, S., & Granhag, P. A. (2011). A comparison between lying about intentions and past activities: Verbal cues and detection accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 212218. Doi 10.1002/acp.1665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A., Leal, S., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2012). Imposing cognitive load to elicit cues to deceit: Inducing the reverse order technique naturally. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 18, 579594. Doi 10.1080/10683160902776843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A., Leal, S., Mann, S., Warmelink, L., Granhag, P. A., & Fisher, R. P. (2010). Drawings as an innovative and successful lie detection tool. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 587594. Doi: 10.1002/acp/1627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A., Mann, S., Fisher, R., Leal, S., Milne, B., & Bull, R. (2008). Increasing cognitive load to facilitate lie detection: The benefit of recalling an event in reverse order. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 253265. Doi 10.1007/s10979-007-9103-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrij, A., Mann, S., Leal, S., & Fisher, R. (2010). “Look into my eyes”: Can an instruction to maintain eye contact facilitate lie detection? Psychology, Crime, & Law, 16, 327348. doi 10.1080/10683160902776843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A., Mann, S., Leal, S., & Fisher, R. (2012). Is anyone out there? Drawings as a tool to detect deception in occupations interviews. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 18, 377388. doi: 10.1080/1068316X.2010.498422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warmelink, L., Vrij, A., Mann, S., Jundi, S., & Granhag, P. A. (2012). Have you been there before? The effect of experience and question expectedness on lying about intentions. Acta Psychologica, 141, 178183. doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.07.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, L. A., Veinott, E. S., Meissner, C. M., Fallon, C., & Mueller, S. T. (2010). Field Training and Testing of Interview and Elicitation Methods (Final Report prepared under Contract W91CRB-09-C-0082 for US ARMY REDCOM Acquisition Center, Aberdeen Proving Ground, MD). Fairborn, OH: Klein Associates Division of Applied Research Associates.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×