Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T01:28:33.845Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Introduction: Writing the History of Social Science

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 March 2008

Theodore M. Porter
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Dorothy Ross
Affiliation:
The Johns Hopkins University
Get access

Summary

How do we write the history of social science? There are problems even with the name. In English alone, “sciences of man,” “moral sciences,” “moral and political sciences,” “behavioral sciences,” and “human sciences” have been among its many predecessors and competitors. Their proliferation reflects the unsettled nature of this broad subject matter. All are capable of giving offense, both by exclusion and by inclusion. Many have long and contradictory histories.

Consider the career of the “moral sciences.” The phrase “sciences morales et politiques” was introduced in France about 1770. In 1795 it was enshrined as the official label for the “second class” of the Institut de France (the former Académie des Sciences was the first class), until this nest of critics was reorganized out of existence by Napoleon in 1803. Restored in 1832, the official institution of the moral and political sciences was now suitably conservative, emphasizing philosophy and individual morality. John Stuart Mill, an admirer of Auguste Comte’s “sociology,” included in his enduringly influential 1843 treatise on logic a section aiming to “remedy” the “backward state of the moral sciences” by “applying to them the methods of physical science, duly extended and generalized.” A German translation of Mill’s work rendered “moral sciences” as Geisteswissenschaften – not the first use of that German term, but an influential one. It referred to the sciences of Geist, which could be translated back into English as “spirit” or “mind.” In German, this remained a standard label until well into the twentieth century. It was understood to indicate that such studies had a moral and spiritual character, quite unlike the sciences of nature.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ash, Mitchell G.The Self-Presentation of a Discipline: History of Psychology in the United States between Pedagogy and Scholarship,” in Functions and Uses of Disciplinary Histories, ed. Graham, Loren, Lepenies, Wolf, and Weingart, Peter (Boston: D. Reidel, 1983)Google Scholar
Benschop, Ruth and Draaisma, Douwe, “In Pursuit of Precision: The Calibration of Minds and Machines in Late Nineteenth-Century Psychology,” Annals of Science, 57 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnham, John C. since collected in Paths into American Culture (Philadelphia: Temple University Press, 1988)Google Scholar
Coats, A. W.Research Priorities in the History of Economics,” all in History of Political Economy, 1 (Spring 1969).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danziger, , Constructing the Subject (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furner, Mary O.Advocacy and Objectivity: A Crisis in the Professionalization of American Social Science, 1865–1905 (Lexington: University Press of Kentucky, 1975).Google Scholar
Gutting, Gary, ed., Paradigms and Revolutions: Appraisals and Applications of Thomas Kuhn’s Philosophy of Science (Notre Dame, Ind.: University of Notre Dame Press, 1980).Google Scholar
Hale, Nathan G. Jr., Freud and the Americans: The Beginnings of Psychoanalysis in the United States, 1876–1917 (New York: Oxford University Press, 1971).Google Scholar
Haller, Mark H.Eugenics: Hereditarian Attitudes in American Thought (New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press, 1963)Google Scholar
Hayden, WhiteThe Content of the Form: Narrative Discourse and Historical Representation (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1987)Google Scholar
Kuhn, ThomasThe Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 2nd ed. enlarged (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1970)Google Scholar
Morawski, Jill, ed., The Rise of Experimentation in American Psychology (New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1988)Google Scholar
Nikolas, RoseThe Psychological Complex: Psychology, Politics and Society in England, 1869–1939 (London: Routledge, 1985).Google Scholar
Peter, NovickThat Noble Dream: The “Objectivity Question” and the American Historical Profession (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1988).Google Scholar
Porter, Theodore M.The Rise of Statistical Thinking, 1820–1900 (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1986)Google Scholar
Skinner, Quentin, ed., The Return of Grand Theory in the Human Sciences (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1985).Google Scholar
Smith, RogerThe Fontana History of the Human Sciences (London: Fontana Press, 1997).Google Scholar
Stocking, George W. Jr., “On the Limits of ‘Presentism’ and ‘Historicism’ in the Historiography of the Behavioral Sciences” (1965), in his Race, Culture, and Evolution: Essays in the History of Anthropology (New York: Free Press, 1968)Google Scholar
Vidal, FernandoLa ‘science de l’homme’: Désirs d’unité et juxtapositions encyclopédiques,” in L’Histoire des sciences de l’homme: Trajectoire, enjeux et questions vives, ed. Blanckaert, Claude, Blondiaux, Loïc, Loty, Laurent, Renneville, Marc, and Richard, Nathalie (Paris: L’Harmattan, 1999)Google Scholar
Wise, M. NortonWork and Waste: Political Economy and Natural Philosophy in Nineteenth-Century Britain,” History of Science, 27 (1989), and 28 (1990)Google Scholar
Young, RobertDarwin’s Metaphor: Nature’s Place in Victorian Culture (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1970)Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×