Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-pkt8n Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-18T17:28:43.816Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Item response theory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 January 2010

Howard Wainer
Affiliation:
National Board of Medical Examiners, Philadelphia
Eric T. Bradlow
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Xiaohui Wang
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
Get access

Summary

Item response theory (IRT) is a family of mathematical descriptions of what happens when an examinee meets an item. It stems from early notions that test items all ought to somehow measure the same thing (Loevinger, 1947). Originally, IRT formalized this by explicitly positing a single dimension of knowledge or underlying trait on which all examinees rely, to some extent, for their correct responses to all the test items (although recent research has generalized this to allow multidimensionality (Reckase, 1985, 1997; McDonald, 2000; Segall, 2001)). Examples of such traits are verbal proficiency, mathematical facility, jumping ability, or spatial memory. The position that each item occupies on this dimension is termed that item's difficulty (usually denoted as b); the position of each examinee on this dimension is that examinee's proficiency (usually denoted as θ). The IRT model gives the probability of answering a question correctly in terms of the difference between b and θ (both of which are unobservable). The simplest IRT model combines just these two elements within a logistic function. Because it characterizes each item with just a single parameter (difficulty= b), in its logistic form it is called the one parameter logistic model or 1-PL. This model was first developed and popularized by the Danish mathematician Georg Rasch (1901–1980), and so is often termed the Rasch Model in his honor. We shall denote it as the 1-PL model in this account to reinforce its position as a member of a parametric family of logistic models.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×