Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-fnpn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-25T20:27:50.014Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

93 - Closing the Divide: Psychological Science, Basic and Applied

from Section B - Understanding Mental Disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

When I began graduate study of psychology in the early 1950s, I entered a theoretically divided house. Most experimental psychologists could be described as neo-behaviorists, studying classical and operant conditioning, paired associate learning, psychophysics, and the like. The data collected were measures of physical events in animals and humans. Theories described connections between objective measures, as in other physical sciences; inferences about mediating mental phenomena (hypothetical constructs) were highly controversial and prompted vigorous debate.

Clinical psychology was, however, a very different matter.

Based on the great need for mental health workers, occasioned by the flood of psychiatric casualties among war-veterans in the aftermath of World War II, an increasing proportion of university psychologists were treating patients and training students in clinical practice. The then general view of mental disorder was, as for psychiatry, dominated by the theories of Sigmund Freud, his colleagues, and disciples.

In the 1950s, clinical evaluation did not involve the measurement of behavior or physiology, but depended on analysis of patients’ symptom reports and personal memories, gathered in a prolonged series of interviews – a “talking cure.” The therapist's interpretations were intended to uncover patients’ hidden (unconscious) foci of distress. Treatment was achieved when the patients gained emotional “insight”: a conscious realization of the dark motives determining their dysfunctional behavior.

In graduate school, I took the sequence of clinical courses and associated practicum, along with the required program in experimental psychology. I vividly recall a curious sense of “split-personality” that developed during these studies. In experimental psychology, careful objective measurement, statistical analysis, and rigorous theory-testing characterized one-half of my days; for the clinical half, I was like an acolyte training for a new priesthood, learning to sense the presence of frightening, invisible forces, buried in patients’ monologues.

The trajectory of my subsequent work as a scientist, I believe, stemmed from the initial uneasiness with this divide – that there could be two psychologies, casually tolerated by both camps, so different in how knowledge was acquired – with fundamentally opposed views as to what constituted meaningful data.

In 1958, I joined the psychology faculty at the University of Pittsburgh. In that same year, a book was published that had a profound effect on my future research career: Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition, written by Joseph Wolpe.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 445 - 450
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Lang, P. J., McTeague, L., & Bradley, M. M. (in press). RDoC, DSM, and the reflex physiology of fear: A biodimensional analysis of the anxiety disorders spectrum. Psychophysiology.
Pavlov, I. P. (1936). Bequest of Pavlov (written just before his death; translator Kupalov, P.). In Flakeslee, A. F., Discussion. Science, 83, 369–370.Google Scholar
Wolpe, J., (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition, Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×