Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-pkt8n Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-15T02:06:25.123Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Temperamental and Emotional Influences on Peer Relationships

from Commentary I

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2009

Joan Stevenson-Hinde
Affiliation:
Cambridge University High St., Madingley Cambridge, U.K.
Xinyin Chen
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
Doran C. French
Affiliation:
Illinois Wesleyan University
Barry H. Schneider
Affiliation:
University of Ottawa
Get access

Summary

Following on from the book's first section concerning theoretical and methodological issues, the three chapters in this section focus on “within-individual” influences on peer relationships, as a logical prelude to the final sections concerning interactions with parents and peers. This organization reflects the levels of analysis outlined by Hinde (1987), which range from the individual to interactions, relationships, groups, and society (Fig. 9.1; further described in Chapter 6, p. 128). As indicated by the bidirectional arrows in Figure 9.1, each level influences and is influenced by sociocultural structure, with its beliefs, values, conventions, and institutions. Chen, Wang, and DeSouza's model (Chapter 6, Fig. 9.1) elaborates on this by considering specific individual characteristics in relation to their value in self-oriented versus group-oriented cultures. Particular characteristics, such as shyness–inhibition, are arranged on two main dimensions, namely social initiative and self-control. These are considered to be manifestations in the social domain of Rothbart's fundamental temperamental dimensions, namely reactivity and regulation (Rothbart & Bates, 1998).

Within Rothbart's framework, behavioral assessments include activity, duration of orienting, distress to limitations, smiling, soothability, and fear. Indeed, all the major theories of childhood temperament contain a dimension related to fearfulness (reviewed in Goldsmith et al., 1987), a trait recognized over many years (e.g., Kagan, 1994), in many species (Gosling & John, 1999), including other primates (Stevenson-Hinde, Stillwell-Barnes, & Zunz, 1980). In addition, fearfulness is a particularly salient individual characteristic in childhood, when increasing exploration beyond the family leads to encounters with unfamiliar people and events.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & IJzendoorn, M. H. (2004). No association of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and -521 C/T promoter polymorphisms with infant attachment disorganization. Attachment & Human Development, 6, 211–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 195–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bokhorst, C. L.,Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J.,Fearon, R. M. P.,IJzendoorn, M. H.,Fonagy, P.,Schuengel, C. (2003). The importance of shared environment in mother-infant attachment security: A behavioral genetic study. Child Development, 74, 1769–1782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss, Vol. I: Attachment. London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1991). Postscript. In Parkes, C. M., Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Marris, P. (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 293–297). London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations among infant temperament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at twenty-four months. Child Development, 63, 1456–1472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J., & Rickman, M. (1999). Behavioral inhibition and the emotional circuitry of the brain. In Schmidt, L. A. & Schulkin, J. (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and social phobia (pp. 67–87). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeWolff, M. S., & IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571–591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., et al, . (1987). Roundtable: What is temperament?Child Development, 58, 505–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals: A cross-species review. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 69–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, K., Fremer-Bombik, E., Rudolph, J., & Grossmann, K. E. (1988). Maternal attachment representation as related to patterns of infant-mother attachment and maternal care during the first year. In Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (Eds.), Relationships within families (pp. 241–262). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1987). Individuals, relationships, and culture. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1991). Perspectives on attachment. In Parkes, C. M., Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Marris, P. (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 52–65). London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, P., & Meng, Z. (1996). An examination of infant-mother attachment in China. Poster presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Québec City, Canada.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1994). Galen's prophecy. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lesch, K.-P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, S. et al. (1996). Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274, 1527–1531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, P. J., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (2001). Behavioral inhibition: Physiological correlates. In Crozier, W. R. & Alden, L. E. (Eds.), International handbook of social anxiety (pp. 53–76). Chichester, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
Miyake, K., Chen, S. J., & Campos., J. J. (1985). Infant temperament, mother's mode of interactions, and attachment in Japan: An interim report. In Bretherton, I. & Waters, E. (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209), 276–297.Google Scholar
Mizuta, I., Zahn-Waxler, C., Cole, P. M., & Hiruma, N. (1996). A cross-cultural study of preschoolers' attachments: Security and sensitivity in Japanese and US dyads. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 141–159.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1998). DNA and personality. European Journal of Personality, 12, 387–407.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., & Rutter, M. (1998). Child development, molecular genetics, and what to do with genes once they are found. Child Development, 69, 1223–1242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Ed.) and Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 105–176). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Schmidt, L., Polak, C. P., & Spooner, A. L. (2001). Biological and environmental contributions to childhood shyness: A diathesis-stress model. In Crozier, W. R. & Alden, L. E. (Eds.), International handbook of social anxiety (pp. 29–51). Chichester, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
Stevenson-Hinde, J. (2005). On the interplay between attachment, temperament, and maternal style. In Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., & Waters, E. (Eds.). The power and dynamics of longitudinal attachment research. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Marshall, P. J. (1999). Behavioral inhibition, heart period, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia: An attachment perspective. Child Development, 70, 805–816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson-Hinde, J., Stillwell-Barnes, R., & Zunz, M. (1980). Subjective assessment of rhesus monkeys over four successive years. Primates, 21, 66–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2004). Maternal sensitivity and infant temperament in the formation of attachment (pp. 233–258). In Bremner, G. & Slater, A. (Eds.), Theories of infant development. London: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Sagi, A. (1999). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual dimensions. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 713–734). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. A. (1999). The nature of individual differences in infant-caregiver attachment. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 68–88). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Zevalkink, J. (1997). Attachment in Indonesia: The mother-child relationship in context. Doctoral dissertation, University of Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (ISBN 90–9010829–7).Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×