Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-12T14:36:58.499Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Schemes for schemata

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Janet Dixon Keller
Affiliation:
University of Illinois, Urbana
Theodore Schwartz
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
Catherine A. Lutz
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Get access

Summary

Anthropologists have contributed to the study of mind throughout this century. Yet as the discipline has developed, approaches to the study of mind have diversified and communication among scholars with disparate approaches has become increasingly difficult. It is useful at this point in our history to reinstate the more inclusive vision of an earlier time and reopen lines of communication. Ultimately we all focus on some central questions. What are the formal and substantive properties of mind? What metaphysical assumptions must we make to account for particular hypothesized states of mind? To what extent are we dealing with a modular system?

With these questions in view cognitive anthropologists focus on knowledge representation and inference. The key term is schema. It replaces the blank slate and the black box, accounting for order, inference, and directive force. Yet the concept of schema has introduced at least as many problems as it has helped resolve. Schema: what is it? D'Andrade, building on Fillmore's (1975) research, argues that a schema is an organization of knowledge which constitutes a simplified world used to reason with. To elaborate just a little, schemata are culturally derived, knowledge-generated structures which facilitate comprehension and inferencing (Hutchins 1980; Nuckolls 1986). What constitutes a well-formed schema either formally or substantively? At this point we can't answer this question. The classic cognitive perspective in anthropology thought it could. Knowledge structures early in the history of cognitive anthropology were thought to consist of paradigms and taxonomies which in turn consisted of conceptual nodes defined by etic features in emic combinations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×