Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-n9wrp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T09:28:15.525Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - A multi-component system that constructs knowledge: Insights from microgenetic study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Deanna Kuhn
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University
Nira Granott
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Boston
Get access

Summary

As the appearance of a volume like the present one attests, the growing use of the microgenetic method is in the process of transforming developmental research, focusing it on its true subject – change. Cross-sectional “snapshots” are not just limited in what they portray. More seriously, they may be misleading, since an individual's second encounter with a task may reveal an entirely different approach from a first encounter. The “dynamic assessment” over time that goes back to Vygotsky provides a more informative picture of how an individual functions. Extended over a longer time period, dynamic assessment merges with the microgenetic method. Strategies evolve with the exercise that comes from extended engagement, allowing observation of the change process – a process that presumably would take place in a similar way, although at a slower pace, in the absence of this dense experience.

In this pure form, the microgenetic method allows examination of behavior as it is reorganized simply as a consequence of its own functioning – a process we can assume is a common one in natural settings, since a great many behaviors do change in the absence of instruction or explicit feedback. In this form of the microgenetic method, which my co-workers and I began to use in the early 1980s as a means of studying mechanisms of change (Kuhn & Ho, 1980; Kuhn & Phelps, 1982), the only feedback individuals receive is that arising from their own actions.

Type
Chapter
Information
Microdevelopment
Transition Processes in Development and Learning
, pp. 109 - 130
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersen, C. (1998). A microgenetic study of science reasoning in social context. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Teachers College, Columbia University, New York
Bijou, S. (1976). Child development: The basic stage of early childhood. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Bransford, J., Brown, A., & Cocking, R. (Eds.). (1999). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. Report of the National Research Council. Washington, DC: National Academy Press
Brown, A. (1997). Transforming schools into communities of thinking and learning about serious matters. American Psychologist, 52, 399–413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, C., Burtis, J., & Bereiter, C. (1997). Knowledge-building as a mediator of conflict in conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 15, 1–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Z., & Siegler, R. (in press). Across the great divide: Bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers’ and older children's thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
Crowley, K., & Siegler, R. (1999). Explanation and generalization in young children's strategy learning. Child Development, 70, 304–316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jong, T., & Joolingen, W. R. (1998). Scientific discovery learning with computer simulations of conceptual domains. Review of Educational Research, 68(2), 179–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenhart, M., Finkel, E., & Marion, S. (1996). Creating the conditions for scientific literacy: A re-examination. American Educational Research Journal, 33, 261–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felton, M., & Kuhn, D. (in press). The development of argumentive discourse skills. Discourse Processes
Gardner, H. (1999). The disciplined mind. New York: Simon & Schuster
Granott, N. (1998). We learn, therefore we develop: Learning versus development – or developing learning? In C. Smith & T. Pourchot (Eds.), Adult learning and development: Perspectives from educational psychology (pp. 15–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Granott, N. (this volume). How microdevelopment creates macrodevelopment: Reiterated sequences, backward transitions, and the Zone of Current Development
Harman, G. (1986). Change in view: Principles of reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Hofer, B., & Pintrich, P. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67, 88–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koslowski, B. (1996). Theory and evidence: The development of scientific reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Kuhn, D. (1983). On the dual executive and its significance in the development of developmental psychology. In D. Kuhn & J. Meacham (Eds.), On the development of developmental psychology. Basel: Karger
Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674–689CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. New York: Cambridge University Press
Kuhn, D. (1995a). Microgenetic study of change: What has it told us?Psychological Science, 6, 133–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D. (Ed.). (1995b). Development and learning: Reconceptualizing the intersection. Human Development (Special issue), 38(6)
Kuhn, D. (1999). Metacognitive development. In L. Balter & C. Tamis Le-Monda (Ed.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press
Kuhn, D., Black, J., Keselman, A., & Kaplan, D. (2000). The development of cognitive skills to support inquiry learning. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 495–523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., & Andersen, C. (1995). Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(4) (Serial No. 245)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., & Ho, V. (1980). Self-directed activity and cognitive development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 1, 119–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., & Pearsall, S. (1998). Relations between metastrategic knowledge and strategic performance. Cognitive Development, 13, 227–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., & Pearsall, S. (2000). Developmental origins of scientific thinking. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 113–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., & Phelps, E. (1982). The development of problem-solving strategies. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 17. New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Kuhn, D., Schauble, L., & Garcia-Mila, M. (1992). Cross-domain development of scientific reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 9(4), 285–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., Shaw, V., & Felton, M. (1997). Effects of dyadic interaction on argumentive reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 15, 287–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., & Weinstock, M. (in press). What is epistemological thinking and why does it matter? In B. Hofer & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
McGinn, M., & Roth, W. (1999). Preparing students for competent scientific practice: implications of recent research in science and technology studies. Educational Researcher, 28, 14–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearsall, S. (1999). The influence of metacognitive reflection on the development of scientific reasoning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York
Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schauble, L. (1996). The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Developmental Psychology, 32, 102–119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoenfeld, A. (1999). Looking toward the 21st century: Challenges of educational theory and practice. Educational Researcher, 28, 4–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1994). Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 1–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children's thinking. New York: Oxford University Press
Siegler, R. S. (this volume). Microgenetic studies of self-explanation
Sophian, C. (1997). Beyond competence: The significance of performance for conceptual development. Cognitive Development, 12, 281–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, S. (1999). Creating the conditions for cognitive change: The interaction between task structures and specific strategies. Child Development, 70, 588–603CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (5th edn., pp. 523–574). New York: Wiley

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×