Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-g5fl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-28T16:21:57.086Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2010

Earl Hunt
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L., and Beier, M. D. (2001). Trait complexes, cognitive investment, and domain knowledge. In Sternberg, R. J. and Grigorenko, Elena L. (eds.). The Psychology of Abilities, Competences, and Expertise. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–30.Google Scholar
Alroy, J. (2001). A multispecies overkill simulation of the end-pliestocene megafaunal mass extinction. Science, 292(8 June): 1893–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, N. H. (1996). A Functional Theory of Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anonymous. ([1100s] 1959). El Poema de mio Cid. Translated by Merwin, W. S.. London: Dent.Google Scholar
Arrow, K. J. (1963). Social Choice and Individual Values. 2d ed. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., and Perrin, N. A. (1988). Toward a unified theory of similarity and recognition. Psychological Review, 95(1): 124–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, H. P., and Hall, L. K. (1991). Lifetime maintenance of high school mathematics content. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120(1): 20–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barzelon, E. (2005). Sentencing by the numbers. New York Times Magazine, January 2.Google Scholar
Beach, L. R. (1990). Image Theory: Decision Making in Personal and Organizational Contexts. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bernstein, P. L. (1996). Against the Gods: The Remarkable Story of Risk. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Committee to Review Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph (2003). The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Crystal, D. (ed.) (1995). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dardonini, V. (2001). A pedagogical proof of Arrow's Impossibility Theorem. Social Choice and Welfare, 18: 107–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiSessa, A. A. (2000). Changing Minds: Computers, Learning, and Literacy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dodds, P. S., Muhamad, R., and Watts, D. J. (2003). An experimental study of search in global social networks. Science, 301(8 August): 827–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, J. P. (1975). Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Embretson, S., and Reise, S. (2000). Item Response Theory for Psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Emmeche, C. (1994). The Garden in the Machine: The Emerging Science of Artificial Life. Translated by Sampson, S.. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Falmagne, J-C. (1985). Elements of Psychophysical Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive Thinking: Rationality in the Real World. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2000). The Tipping Point. New York: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a New Science. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24(1): 79–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottman, J. M., Murray, J. D., Swanson, C. C., Tyson, R., & Swanson, K. R. (2002). The Mathematics of Marriage: Dynamic Non-linear Models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed. American Psychologist, 47(6): 766–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullberg, J. (1997). Mathematics from the Birth of Numbers. London: Charles Norton.Google Scholar
Halpern, D. (2002). Thought and Knowledge: An Introduction to Critical Thinking. 4th ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1996). Human Judgment and Social Policy: Irreducible Uncertainty, Inevitable Error, Unavoidable Injustice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (2001). The weatherman. American Scientist, 89(1): 10–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (2002a). Statistics of deadly quarrels. American Scientist, 90(1): 10–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (2002b). Follow the money. American Scientist, 90(5): 400–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (2002c). Reply to letters to the editor. American Scientist, 90(6): 494.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Henley, N. M. (1969). A psychological study of the semantics of animal terms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8(2): 176–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J., and Murray, C. (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hoffrage, U., and Gigerenzer, G. (1996). The impact of information representation on Bayesian reasoning. Proc. of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 126–30.Google Scholar
Huillard d'Aignaux, J. N., Cousens, S. N., and Smith, P. G. (2001). Predictability of the UK variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease epidemic. Science, 294(23 November): 1729–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. (1995). Will We Be Smart Enough? A Cognitive Analysis of the Coming Workforce. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (2002). Thoughts on Thought. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hunt, M. M. (1993). The Story of Psychology. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., and Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating conscious and unconscious influences on memory: Measuring recollections. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(2): 139–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Westport, CT: Praeger/Greenwood.Google Scholar
Jochem, T., and Pomerleau, D. (1996). Life in the fast lane: The evolution of an adaptive vehicle control system. AI Magazine, 17(2): 11–50.Google Scholar
Johnson, W., and Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2005). The structure of intelligence. It's verbal, perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not fluid and crystallized. Intelligence, 33(4): 393–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, W., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., and Gottesman, I. I. (2004). Just one g: Consistent results from three test batteries. Intelligence, 32: 95–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hove, UK: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9): 697–720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. A., and Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80(4): 237–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory. Econometrica, 47: 263–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, G. (1998). Sources of Power: How People Make Decisions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (2002). The Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Link, S. W. (1994). Rediscovering the past: Gustav Fechner and signal detection theory. Psychological Science, 5: 335–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. (1986). Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., and Galanter, E. (1963). Discrimination. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., and Galanter, E. (eds.). Handbook of Mathematical Psychology: Volume 1. New York: Wiley, pp. 191–244.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., and Krumhansl, C. L. (1988). Measurement, scaling, and psychophysics. In Atkinson, R. C. and Herrnstein, R. J. (eds.). Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 1: Perception and Motivation. New York: Wiley, pp. 3–74.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., and Raiffa, H. (1957). Games and Decisions: Introduction and Critical Survey. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lyman, R. (2002). Letter to the editor. American Scientist, 90(6): 494.Google Scholar
MacMillan, N. A., and Creelman, C. D. (2004). Detection Theory: A User's Guide. 2d ed. Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88: 375–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milgram, S. (1967). The small world problem. Psychology Today, 1(1): 60–7.Google Scholar
Minsky, M., and Papert, S. (1969). Perceptrons: An Introduction to Computational Geometry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, M. (2002). Doing science: A new kind of science. Science, 298(14 October): 65–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, J. D. (2001). An Introduction to Mathematical Biology. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Narens, L. (2002). The irony of measurement by subjective estimations. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46: 769–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. R. (1956). The World of Mathematics. Vol. 2. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Parducci, A., and Wedell, D. H. (1986). The category effect with rating scales: Number of categories, number of stimuli, and method of presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(4): 496–516.Google ScholarPubMed
Peter, L. J. (1977). Peter's Quotations: Ideas for Our Time. New York: Murrow.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Spieler, D., and McKoon, G. (2000). Explicitly modeling the effects of aging on response time. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7(1) 1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rausch, J. (2002). Seeing around corners. Atlantic Monthly, April, pp. 35–48.Google Scholar
Reich, R. (1991). The Work of Nations: Preparing Ourselves for 21st Century Capitalism. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Richardson, L. F., and Pear, P. (1946). Psychological Factors of Peace and War. London: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C., and Wenzel, A. E. (1996). One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description of retention. Psychological Review, 103(4): 734–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffman, S. S., Reynolds, M. L., and Young, F. W. (1981). Introduction to Multidimensional Scaling: Theory, Methods and Application. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1981). The Sciences of the Artificial. 2d ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1904). General intelligence, objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15: 201–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1927). The Abilities of Man. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Stankov, L. (1999). Mining on the “no man's land” between intelligence and personality. In Ackerman, P. L., Kyllonen, P. C., and Roberts, R. D. (eds.). Learning and Individual Differences: Process, Trait, and Content Determinants. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64: 153–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. (1966). A metric for the social consensus. Science, 151(14 January): 530–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to Its Perceptual, Neural and Social Prospects. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. F. (1993). The Brain: A Neuroscience Primer. 2d ed. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1927). A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Review, 34: 273–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T., and Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Travers, J., and Milgram, S. (1969). An experimental study of the small world problem. Sociometry, 32(4): 425–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the rationality of choice. Science, 211(30 January): 453–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional vs. intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 91: 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vilenkin, N. Y. (1965/1968). Stories about Sets. Scripta Technica Translation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Valleron, A-J., Boelle, P-Y., Will, R., and Cesbron, J-Y. (2001). Estimation of epidemic size and incubation time based on age characteristics of vCJD in the United Kingdom. Science, 294(23 November): 1726–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, J., and Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of Games and Economic Behavior. 2d ed. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wagenaar, W. A. (1986). My memory: A study of autobiographical memory over six years. Cognitive Psychology, 18(2): 225–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K. (1991). Managerial problem solving. In Sternberg, R. J. and Frensch, P. A (eds.). Complex Problem Solving: Principles and Mechanisms. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 159–84.Google Scholar
Williams, G. P. (1997). Chaos Theory Tamed. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Wolfram, S. (2002). A New Kind of Science. Champaign, IL: Wolfram Media.Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., and Beier, M. D. (2001). Trait complexes, cognitive investment, and domain knowledge. In Sternberg, R. J. and Grigorenko, Elena L. (eds.). The Psychology of Abilities, Competences, and Expertise. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–30.Google Scholar
Alroy, J. (2001). A multispecies overkill simulation of the end-pliestocene megafaunal mass extinction. Science, 292(8 June): 1893–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, N. H. (1996). A Functional Theory of Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anonymous. ([1100s] 1959). El Poema de mio Cid. Translated by Merwin, W. S.. London: Dent.Google Scholar
Arrow, K. J. (1963). Social Choice and Individual Values. 2d ed. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., and Perrin, N. A. (1988). Toward a unified theory of similarity and recognition. Psychological Review, 95(1): 124–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, H. P., and Hall, L. K. (1991). Lifetime maintenance of high school mathematics content. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120(1): 20–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barzelon, E. (2005). Sentencing by the numbers. New York Times Magazine, January 2.Google Scholar
Beach, L. R. (1990). Image Theory: Decision Making in Personal and Organizational Contexts. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bernstein, P. L. (1996). Against the Gods: The Remarkable Story of Risk. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Committee to Review Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph (2003). The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Crystal, D. (ed.) (1995). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dardonini, V. (2001). A pedagogical proof of Arrow's Impossibility Theorem. Social Choice and Welfare, 18: 107–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiSessa, A. A. (2000). Changing Minds: Computers, Learning, and Literacy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dodds, P. S., Muhamad, R., and Watts, D. J. (2003). An experimental study of search in global social networks. Science, 301(8 August): 827–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, J. P. (1975). Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Embretson, S., and Reise, S. (2000). Item Response Theory for Psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Emmeche, C. (1994). The Garden in the Machine: The Emerging Science of Artificial Life. Translated by Sampson, S.. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Falmagne, J-C. (1985). Elements of Psychophysical Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive Thinking: Rationality in the Real World. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2000). The Tipping Point. New York: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a New Science. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24(1): 79–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottman, J. M., Murray, J. D., Swanson, C. C., Tyson, R., & Swanson, K. R. (2002). The Mathematics of Marriage: Dynamic Non-linear Models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed. American Psychologist, 47(6): 766–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullberg, J. (1997). Mathematics from the Birth of Numbers. London: Charles Norton.Google Scholar
Halpern, D. (2002). Thought and Knowledge: An Introduction to Critical Thinking. 4th ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1996). Human Judgment and Social Policy: Irreducible Uncertainty, Inevitable Error, Unavoidable Injustice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (2001). The weatherman. American Scientist, 89(1): 10–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (2002a). Statistics of deadly quarrels. American Scientist, 90(1): 10–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (2002b). Follow the money. American Scientist, 90(5): 400–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (2002c). Reply to letters to the editor. American Scientist, 90(6): 494.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Henley, N. M. (1969). A psychological study of the semantics of animal terms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8(2): 176–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J., and Murray, C. (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hoffrage, U., and Gigerenzer, G. (1996). The impact of information representation on Bayesian reasoning. Proc. of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 126–30.Google Scholar
Huillard d'Aignaux, J. N., Cousens, S. N., and Smith, P. G. (2001). Predictability of the UK variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease epidemic. Science, 294(23 November): 1729–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. (1995). Will We Be Smart Enough? A Cognitive Analysis of the Coming Workforce. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (2002). Thoughts on Thought. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hunt, M. M. (1993). The Story of Psychology. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., and Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating conscious and unconscious influences on memory: Measuring recollections. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(2): 139–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Westport, CT: Praeger/Greenwood.Google Scholar
Jochem, T., and Pomerleau, D. (1996). Life in the fast lane: The evolution of an adaptive vehicle control system. AI Magazine, 17(2): 11–50.Google Scholar
Johnson, W., and Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2005). The structure of intelligence. It's verbal, perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not fluid and crystallized. Intelligence, 33(4): 393–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, W., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., and Gottesman, I. I. (2004). Just one g: Consistent results from three test batteries. Intelligence, 32: 95–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hove, UK: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9): 697–720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. A., and Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80(4): 237–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory. Econometrica, 47: 263–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, G. (1998). Sources of Power: How People Make Decisions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (2002). The Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Link, S. W. (1994). Rediscovering the past: Gustav Fechner and signal detection theory. Psychological Science, 5: 335–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. (1986). Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., and Galanter, E. (1963). Discrimination. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., and Galanter, E. (eds.). Handbook of Mathematical Psychology: Volume 1. New York: Wiley, pp. 191–244.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., and Krumhansl, C. L. (1988). Measurement, scaling, and psychophysics. In Atkinson, R. C. and Herrnstein, R. J. (eds.). Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 1: Perception and Motivation. New York: Wiley, pp. 3–74.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., and Raiffa, H. (1957). Games and Decisions: Introduction and Critical Survey. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lyman, R. (2002). Letter to the editor. American Scientist, 90(6): 494.Google Scholar
MacMillan, N. A., and Creelman, C. D. (2004). Detection Theory: A User's Guide. 2d ed. Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88: 375–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milgram, S. (1967). The small world problem. Psychology Today, 1(1): 60–7.Google Scholar
Minsky, M., and Papert, S. (1969). Perceptrons: An Introduction to Computational Geometry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, M. (2002). Doing science: A new kind of science. Science, 298(14 October): 65–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, J. D. (2001). An Introduction to Mathematical Biology. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Narens, L. (2002). The irony of measurement by subjective estimations. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46: 769–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. R. (1956). The World of Mathematics. Vol. 2. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Parducci, A., and Wedell, D. H. (1986). The category effect with rating scales: Number of categories, number of stimuli, and method of presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(4): 496–516.Google ScholarPubMed
Peter, L. J. (1977). Peter's Quotations: Ideas for Our Time. New York: Murrow.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Spieler, D., and McKoon, G. (2000). Explicitly modeling the effects of aging on response time. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7(1) 1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rausch, J. (2002). Seeing around corners. Atlantic Monthly, April, pp. 35–48.Google Scholar
Reich, R. (1991). The Work of Nations: Preparing Ourselves for 21st Century Capitalism. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Richardson, L. F., and Pear, P. (1946). Psychological Factors of Peace and War. London: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C., and Wenzel, A. E. (1996). One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description of retention. Psychological Review, 103(4): 734–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffman, S. S., Reynolds, M. L., and Young, F. W. (1981). Introduction to Multidimensional Scaling: Theory, Methods and Application. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1981). The Sciences of the Artificial. 2d ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1904). General intelligence, objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15: 201–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1927). The Abilities of Man. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Stankov, L. (1999). Mining on the “no man's land” between intelligence and personality. In Ackerman, P. L., Kyllonen, P. C., and Roberts, R. D. (eds.). Learning and Individual Differences: Process, Trait, and Content Determinants. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64: 153–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. (1966). A metric for the social consensus. Science, 151(14 January): 530–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to Its Perceptual, Neural and Social Prospects. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. F. (1993). The Brain: A Neuroscience Primer. 2d ed. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1927). A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Review, 34: 273–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T., and Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Travers, J., and Milgram, S. (1969). An experimental study of the small world problem. Sociometry, 32(4): 425–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the rationality of choice. Science, 211(30 January): 453–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional vs. intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 91: 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vilenkin, N. Y. (1965/1968). Stories about Sets. Scripta Technica Translation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Valleron, A-J., Boelle, P-Y., Will, R., and Cesbron, J-Y. (2001). Estimation of epidemic size and incubation time based on age characteristics of vCJD in the United Kingdom. Science, 294(23 November): 1726–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, J., and Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of Games and Economic Behavior. 2d ed. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wagenaar, W. A. (1986). My memory: A study of autobiographical memory over six years. Cognitive Psychology, 18(2): 225–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K. (1991). Managerial problem solving. In Sternberg, R. J. and Frensch, P. A (eds.). Complex Problem Solving: Principles and Mechanisms. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 159–84.Google Scholar
Williams, G. P. (1997). Chaos Theory Tamed. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Wolfram, S. (2002). A New Kind of Science. Champaign, IL: Wolfram Media.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Earl Hunt, University of Washington
  • Book: The Mathematics of Behavior
  • Online publication: 04 May 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511618222.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Earl Hunt, University of Washington
  • Book: The Mathematics of Behavior
  • Online publication: 04 May 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511618222.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Earl Hunt, University of Washington
  • Book: The Mathematics of Behavior
  • Online publication: 04 May 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511618222.015
Available formats
×